Educational Psychology Review

, Volume 12, Issue 2, pp 229–246

The Smarter Sex: A Critical Review of Sex Differences in Intelligence

  • Diane F. Halpern
  • Mary L. LaMay
Article

Abstract

Although there are no sex differences in general intelligence, reliable differences are found on some tests of cognitive abilities. Many of the tasks that assess the ability to manipulate visual images in working memory show an advantage for males, whereas many of the tasks that require retrieval from long-term memory and the acquisition and use of verbal information show a female advantage. Large effects favoring males are also found on advanced tests of mathematical achievement, especially with highly select samples. Males are also overrepresented in some types of mental retardation. Effects sizes are variable and often large. These differences are generally found cross-culturally and across the life span. The nature–nurture dichotomy is rejected as an interpretive framework. In light of recent findings that environmental variables alter the biological underpinnings of intelligence and individuals actively participate in creating their environments, we prefer a psychobiosocial model for understanding sex differences in intelligence.

human sex differences intelligence spatial abilities verbal abilities human cognition 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. American Association of University Women. (1992). How Schools Shortchange Girls: The AAUW Report. Marlowe, New York.Google Scholar
  2. Astin, A., Sax, L., Korn, W., and Mahoney, K. (1995). The American Freshman: National Norms for Fall 1995. Higher Education Research Institute, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
  3. Banaji, M. R., and Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic stereotyping. Psych. Sci. 7: 136-141.Google Scholar
  4. Birenbaum, M., Kelly, A. E., and Levi-Keren, M. (1994). Stimulus features and sex differences in mental rotation test performance. Intelligence 19: 51-64.Google Scholar
  5. Brody, N. (1992). Intelligence (2nd ed.). Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  6. Brown, L. N., Lahr, C. J., and Mosley, J. L. (1998). Age and gender-related differences in strategy use for route information: A map-present direction-giving paradigm. Environ. Behav. 30: 123-143.Google Scholar
  7. Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R., Pezaris, E. and Benbow, C. (1995). The influence of spatial ability of gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores across diverse samples. Dev. Psych. 31: 697-705.Google Scholar
  8. Cowell, P. E., Turetsky, B. I., Gur, R. C., Grossman, R. I., Shtasel, D. L., and Gur, R. (1994). Sex differences in aging of the human frontal and temporal lobes. J. Neurosci. 14, 4748-4755.Google Scholar
  9. Crawford, J. R., and Allen, K. M. (1995). The WAIS-R (UK): Basic psychometric properties in an adult UK population. Br. J. Clin. Psych. 34: 237-250.Google Scholar
  10. Dabbs, J. M., Chan, E. L., Strong, R. A., and Milun, R. (1998). Spatial-ability, navigation strategy, and geographic knowledge among men and women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 19: 89-98.Google Scholar
  11. DeFries, J. C., and Gillis, J. J. (1993). Genetics of reading disability. In Plomin, R. and McClearn, G. E. (Eds.), Nature, Nurture and Psychology. American Psychological Association. Washington, DC. pp. 121-145.Google Scholar
  12. Eals, M., and Silverman, I. (1994). The hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: Proximate factors mediating the female advantage in recall of object arrays. Ethol. Sociobiol. 15: 95-105.Google Scholar
  13. Feingold. A. (1996). Cognitive gender differences: Where are they and why are they there? Learn. Indiv. Diff. 8: 25-32.Google Scholar
  14. Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of Mind. Basic, New York.Google Scholar
  15. Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., and Mangun, G. R. (1998). Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind. W. W. Norton, New York.Google Scholar
  16. Geffen, G., Moar, K. J., O'Hanlon, A. P., Clark, C. R., and Geffen, L. B. (1990). Performance measures of 16-to 86-year-old males and females on the Auditory Verbal Learning Test. Clin. Neuropsych. 4: 45-63.Google Scholar
  17. Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional Intelligence. Bantam, New York.Google Scholar
  18. Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., and Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Dev. 58: 539-559.Google Scholar
  19. Gur, R. E., and Gur, R. C. (1990). Gender differences in cerebral blood flow. Schizoph. Bull. 16: 247-254.Google Scholar
  20. Halpern, D. F. (1996). Public policy implications of sex differences in cognitive abilities. Psych. Pub. Pol. Law 2: 561-574.Google Scholar
  21. Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence: Implications for education. Am. Psych. 52: 1091-1102.Google Scholar
  22. Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.). Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.Google Scholar
  23. Hampson, E., and Kimura, D. (1988). Reciprocal effects of hormonal fluctuations on human motor and perceptual-spatial skills. Behav. Neurosci. 102: 456-495.Google Scholar
  24. Hare-Mustin, R. T., and Marecek, J. (1994). Asking the right questions: Feminist psychology and sex differences. Fem. Psych. 4: 531-537.Google Scholar
  25. Hedges, L. V., and Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science 269: 41-45.Google Scholar
  26. Henderson, V. W., Paganini, H. A., Emanuel, C. K., Dunn, M. E., and Buckwalter, J. G. (1994). Estrogen replacement therapy in older women: Comparisons between Alzheimer's disease cases and nondemented control subjects. Arch. Neurol. 51: 896-900.Google Scholar
  27. Henning-Stout, M., and Close-Conoley, J. (1992). Gender: A subtle influence in the culture of the school. In Medway, F. J. and Cafferty, T. P. (Eds.), School Psychology: A Social Psychological Perspective (pp. 113-135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  28. Herlitz, A., Nilsson, L-G., and Baeckman, L. (1997). Gender differences in episodic memory. Mem. Cog. 25: 801-811.Google Scholar
  29. Hollway, W. (1994). Beyond sex differences: A project for feminist psychology. Fem. Psych. 4: 538-546.Google Scholar
  30. Horn, J. L. (1985). Psychometric Studies of Aging and Intelligence. Raven Press, New York.Google Scholar
  31. Huang, J. (1993). An investigation of gender differences in cognitive abilities among Chinese high school students. Person. Indiv. Diff. 15: 717-719.Google Scholar
  32. Hunt, E., Pellegrino, J. W., Frick. R. W., Farr, S. A., and Alderton, D. (1988). The ability to reason about movement in the visual field. Intelligence 12: 77-100.Google Scholar
  33. Hunt, M. (1999). The New Know-Nothings. Transaction, New Brunswick.Google Scholar
  34. Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., and Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Dev. Psych. 27: 236-248.Google Scholar
  35. Jacobs, D. M. Tang, M. X., Stern, Y., Sano, M., Marder, K., Bell, K. L., Schofield, P., Dooneief, G., Gurland, B., and Mayeux, R. (1998). Cognitive function in non-demented older women who took estrogen after menopause. Neurology 50: 368-373.Google Scholar
  36. Jensen, A. R. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? Harv. Edu. Rev. 39: 1-123.Google Scholar
  37. Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability. Praeger Publishers, Westport, CT.Google Scholar
  38. Kampen, D. L. and Sherwin, B. B. (1996). Estradiol is related to visual memory in healthy young men. Behav. Neurosci. 110: 613-617.Google Scholar
  39. Kleinfeld, J. (1998). The Myth that Schools Shortchange Girls: Social Science in the Service of Deception. The Women's Freedom Network, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  40. Larrabee, G. J., and Crook, T. H., III. (1993). Do men show more rapid age-associated decline in simulated everyday verbal memory than do women? Psych. Aging 8: 68-71.Google Scholar
  41. Linn, M. C., and Petersen, A. C. (1986). A meta-analysis of gender differences in spatial ability. Implications for mathematics and science achievement. In Hyde, J. S. and Linn, M. C. (Eds.). The Psychology of Gender: Advances through Meta-Analysis. The Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, pp. 67-101.Google Scholar
  42. Lynn, R. (1994). Sex differences in intelligence and brain size: A paradox resolved. Person. Indiv. Diff. 17: 257-271.Google Scholar
  43. Loring-Meier S., and Halpern, D. F. (1999). Sex differences in visual-spatial working memory: Components of cognitive processing. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 6: 464-471.Google Scholar
  44. Lynn, R. (1998). Has the black-white intelligence difference in the United States been narrowing over time? Person. Indiv. Diff. 25: 801-804.Google Scholar
  45. Makintosh, N. J. (1996). Sex differences in IQ. J. Biosoc. Sci. 28: 559-571.Google Scholar
  46. Masters, M. S., and Sanders, B. (1993). Is the gender difference in mental rotation disappearing? Behav. Gen. 23: 337-341.Google Scholar
  47. Meinz, E. J., and Salthouse, T. A. (1998). Is age kinder to females than to males? Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 5: 56-70.Google Scholar
  48. McGuiness, D. (1976). Sex differences in the organization of perception and cognition. In Lloyd, B. and Archer, J. (Eds.). Exploring Sex Differences. Academic Press., New York. pp. 123-156.Google Scholar
  49. Moore, T. (1967). Language and intelligence: A longitudinal study of the first eight years. Hum. Dev. 10: 88-106.Google Scholar
  50. Mortel, K. F., and Meyer, J. S. (1995). Lack of menopausal estrogen replacement therapy and the risk of dementia. J. Neuropsych. Clin. 2: 334-337.Google Scholar
  51. Ohkura, T., Isse, K., Akazawa, K., Hamamoto, M., Yaoi, Y., and Hagino, N. (1995). Long-term estrogen replacement therapy in female patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type: 7 case reports. Dementia 6: 99-107.Google Scholar
  52. Paganini-Hill, A., Buckwalter, J. G., Logan, C. G., and Henderson, V. W. (1993). Estrogen replacement and Alzheimer's disease in women. Soc. Neurosci. Abst. 19: 1046.Google Scholar
  53. Posner, M. I., and Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of Mind. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
  54. Resnick, S. M. (1993). Sex differences in mental rotations: An effect of time limits? Brain Cog. 21: 71-73.Google Scholar
  55. Resnick, S. M., Metter, E. J., and Zonderman, A. B. (1997). Estrogen replacement therapy and longitudinal decline in visual memory: A possible protective effect? Neurology 49: 1491-1497.Google Scholar
  56. Robinson, N. M., Abbott, R. D., Berninger, V. W., and Busse, J. (1996). The structure of abilities in math-precocious young children: Gender similarities and differences. J. Edu. Psych. 88: 341-352.Google Scholar
  57. Schiff, W., and Oldak, R. (1990). Accuracy of judging time to arrival: Effects of modality, trajectory, and gender. J. Exper. Psych. Hum. Percep. Perform. 16: 303-316.Google Scholar
  58. Schmidt, R., Fazekas, F., Reinhart, B., Kapeller, P., Fazekas, G., Offenbacher, H., Eber, B., Schumacher, M., and Freidl, W. (1996). Estrogen replacement therapy in older women: A neuropsychological and brain MRI study. J. Am. Geri. Soc. 44: 1307-1313.Google Scholar
  59. Shucard, D. W., Shucard, J. L., and Thomas, D. G. (1987). Sex differences in electrophysiological activity in infancy. Possible implications for language development. In Phillips, S. U., Steele, S., and Tanz, C. (Eds.), Language, Gender, and Sex in Comparative Perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 278-295.Google Scholar
  60. Simpkins, J. W., Singh, M., and Bishop, J. (1994). The potential role for estrogen replacement therapy in the treatment of the cognitive decline and neurodegeneration associated with Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol. Aging 15: S195-S197.Google Scholar
  61. Smith, C. A., McCleary, C. A., Murdock, G. A., Wilshire, T. W., Buckwalter, D. K., Bretsky, P., Marmol, L., Gorsuch, R. L., and Buckwalter, J. G. (1999). Lifetime estrogen exposure and cognitive performance in elderly women. Brain Cog. 39: 203-218.Google Scholar
  62. Smith, G. A., and McPhee, K. A. (1987). Performance on a coincidence timing task correlates with intelligence. Intelligence 11: 161-167.Google Scholar
  63. Smolak, L. (1986). Infancy, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.Google Scholar
  64. Stanley, J. C., Benbow, C. P., Brody, L. E., Dauber, S., and Lupkowski, A. (1992). Gender differences in eighty-six nationally standardized aptitude and achievement tests. In Colangelo, N., Assouline, S. G., and Ambroson, D. L. (Eds.), Talent Development, Vol. 1: Proceedings from the 1991 Henry B. and Jocelyn Wallace National Research Symposium on Talent Development. Trillium Press, Unionville, NY, pp. 42-65.Google Scholar
  65. Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. Am. Psych. 52: 613-629.Google Scholar
  66. Sternberg, R. J., Wagner, R. K., Williams, W. M., and Horvath, J. A. (1995). Testing common sense. Am. Psych. 50: 912-927.Google Scholar
  67. Stricker, L. J., Rock, D. A., and Burton, N. W. (1993). Sex differences in predictions of college grades from Scholastic Aptitude scores. J. Edu. Psych. 85: 710-718.Google Scholar
  68. Stumpf, H. (1995). Gender differences in performance on tests of cognitive abilities: Experimental design issues and empirical results. Learn. Indiv. Diff. 7: 275-287.Google Scholar
  69. Stumpf, H., and Eliot, J. (1995). Gender-related differences in spatial ability and the k factor of general spatial ability in a population of academically talented students. Person. Indiv. Diff. 19: 33-45.Google Scholar
  70. Stumpf, H., and Jackson, D. N. (1994). Gender-related differences in cognitive abilities: Evidence from a medical school admissions testing program. Person. Indiv. Diff. 17: 335-344.Google Scholar
  71. Stumpf, H., and Stanley, J. C. (1996). Gender-related differences on the College Board's Advanced Placement and Achievement Tests, 1982-1992. J. Edu. Psych. 88: 353-364.Google Scholar
  72. Swim, J. K. (1994). Perceived versus meta-analytic effect sizes: An assessment of the accuracy of gender stereotypes. J. Person. Soc. Psych. 66: 21-36.Google Scholar
  73. U. S. Department of Education. (1997). National Assessment of Educational Progress (Indicator 32: Writing proficiency: Prepared by the Educational Testing Service). Washington, DC. (World Wide Web: http//www.ed/gov/nces)Google Scholar
  74. Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional brain imaging studies of cortical mechanisms for memory. Science 270: 769-775.Google Scholar
  75. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., and Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psych. Bull. 117: 250-270.Google Scholar
  76. Weiss, L. G., and Prifitera, A. (1995). An evaluation of differential prediction of WIAT achievement scores from WISC-III FSIQ across ethnic and gender groups. J. School Psych. 33: 297-304.Google Scholar
  77. West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psych. Bull. 120: 272-292.Google Scholar
  78. Willingham, W. W., and Cole, N. S. (1997). Gender and Fair Assessment. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Diane F. Halpern
    • 1
  • Mary L. LaMay
    • 2
  1. 1.California State UniversitySan Bernardino
  2. 2.Loma Linda UniversityLoma Linda

Personalised recommendations