Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 14, Issue 1–2, pp 119–143 | Cite as

Nature and importance of the logographic phase in learning to read

  • Mireille Bastien-Toniazzo
  • Sandrine Jullien
Article

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to determine the natureand role of knowledge constructed by the child at thevery beginning of his or her contact with printedwords, i.e., during the logographic phase. The resultsof four experiments conducted with 5-year-oldsprovided no supporting evidence for global processingof words. They did suggest, however, that childrenrely on the letters in words, which act as wordidentifiers. A few letters appear to suffice,particularly if they are rare or in word-initialposition. Moreover, letter order is not processed bysome children. The results as a whole indicate thatthe letters in a word serve as its identifying visualproperties as long as, for the child, the solefunction of the written language is to encode meaning.This phase appears to be crucial in learning to read,since it supplies the precursors upon which lateracquisitions are based.

Cognitive development Learning Logographic phase Reading 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bastien, C. (1997). Les connaissances de l'enfant à l'adulte [Knowledge from the child to the adult]. Paris: Masson &; A. Colin.Google Scholar
  2. Bastien, C. &; Bovet, P. (1975). La découverte du parcours ordonné par l'enfant [The discovery of the ordered course by children], Enfance 3: 123-133.Google Scholar
  3. Bastien, C. &; Bastien-Toniazzo, M. (1993). L'importance de la période dite «logographique» dans l'acquisition de la lecture [Importance of the so-called ‘logographic’ stage in reading acquisition]. In: J-P. Jaffré, L. Sprenger-Charolles &; M. Fayol (eds.), Lecture-écriture: acquisition [Reading-writing: acquisition] (pp. 163-175). Paris: Nathan.Google Scholar
  4. Bastien, C., Pélissier A. &; Tête, A. (1990). An experiment in learning logical negation by 6-and-7 year old children, C.P.C./European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 10: 43-53.Google Scholar
  5. Bastien-Toniazzo, M. (1992). La représentation du mot écrit aux tout débuts de la lecture [The representation of the written word at the beginning of reading], L'Année Psychologique [The Psychological Year] 92: 489-509.Google Scholar
  6. Bastien-Toniazzo, M. (1995). L'importance de l'ordre des lettres dans l'apprentissage de la lecture [Importance of the order of letters in reading acquisition], Revue Française de Pédagogie [French Review of Pedagogy] 113: 51-58.Google Scholar
  7. Bastien-Toniazzo, M. (1997). Tutorials in domain-specific acquisition, International Journal of Psychology 32: 129-138.Google Scholar
  8. Beau, A.S. (1994). Identification du mot chez l'adulte analphabète [Word identification by illiterate adult], Master's thesis in psychology, Université de Provence.Google Scholar
  9. Blanchet, A. (1992). Les unités procédurales, causales et téléonomiques dans l'étude des processus cognitifs [Procedural, causal and teleonomical units in cognitive processes studies]. In: B. Inhelder &; G. Cellerier (eds.), Le cheminement des découvertes chez l'enfant [The pathway of the discoveries in child] (pp. 93-118). Neuchâtel-Paris: Delachaux &; Niestlé.Google Scholar
  10. Bovet, P. &; Bastien, C. (1978). Un modèle de résolution de problèmes portant sur des ensembles définis en extension: le schème de l'ensemble ordonné [A problem solving model about sets extension-defined: the sheme of the ordered set], Cahiers de Psychologie 21: 53-64.Google Scholar
  11. Bradley, L. (1988). Making connections in learning to read and spell, Applied Cognitive Psychology 2: 3-18.Google Scholar
  12. Bradley, L. &; Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: a causal connection, Nature 301: 419-421.Google Scholar
  13. Cellerier, G. (1992). Organisation et fonctionnement des schèmes [Schemes' organization and running]. In: B. Inhelder &; G. Cellerier (eds.), Le cheminement des découvertes de l'enfant [The pathway of the discoveries in child] (pp. 255-302). Neuchâtel-Paris: Delachaux &; Niestlé.Google Scholar
  14. Courrieu, P. (1985). Des lettres sans position dans la perception des mots [Unpositioned letters in words perception], L'Année Psychologique [The Psychological Year] 85: 9-25.Google Scholar
  15. Cossu, G., Gugliotta, M. &; Marshall, J.C. (1995). Acquisition of reading and written spelling in a transparent orthography: two non-parallel processing, Reading and Wrting: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7: 9-22.Google Scholar
  16. Cunningham, T.F., Healy, A.F., Kanengiser, N. &; Cizzick, L. (1988). Investigating the boundaries of reading units across ages and reading levels, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 45: 175-208.Google Scholar
  17. Deloche, G., Debili, F. &; Andreewsky, E. (1980). Order information redundancy of verbal codes in French and in English: neurolinguistic implications, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19: 525-530.Google Scholar
  18. Ehri, L.C. &; Wilce, L.S. (1985). Movement into reading: is the first stage of printed word learning visual or phonetic? Reading Research Quarterly 20: 163-179.Google Scholar
  19. Ehri, L.C. (1989). Apprendre à lire et écrire les mots [Learning to read and write words]. In: L. Rieben &; C. Perfetti (eds.), L'apprenti lecteur [The beginning reader] (pp. 103-127). Neuchâtel-Paris: Delachaux &; Niestlé.Google Scholar
  20. Ellis, N. &; Large, B. (1988). The early stage of reading: a longitudinal study, Applied Cognitive Psychology 2: 47-76.Google Scholar
  21. Foureaux, F. (1988). L'accès à l'écrit chez le pré-lecteur: nature et traitement des indices prélevés [Entering into writing: nature of the cues], Doctoral thesis, Université de Provence.Google Scholar
  22. Frith, U. (1986). A developmental framework for developmental dyslexia, Annals of Dyslexia 36: 69-81.Google Scholar
  23. Gough, P.B.&; Hillinger, M.L. (1980). Learning to read: an unnatural act, Bulletin of the Orton Society 30: 171-176.Google Scholar
  24. Harris, M. &; Coltheart, M. (1986). Language processing in children and adults: an introduction. London: Routledge &; Kegan.Google Scholar
  25. Huang, H.S. &; Hanley, J.R. (1994). Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English, Cognition 54: 73-98.Google Scholar
  26. Inhelder, B. &; Cellerier, G. (eds.) (1992). Le cheminement des découvertes de l'enfant [The pathway of the discoveries in child], Neuchâtel-Paris: Delachaux &; Niestlé.Google Scholar
  27. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  28. Lecocq, P., 1991, Apprentissage de la lecture et dyslexie [Reading acquisition and dyslexia]. Brussels: Mardaga.Google Scholar
  29. Lunney, G.H. (1970). Using analysis of variance with a dichotomous dependent variable: an empirical study, Journal of Educational Measurement 7: 263-269.Google Scholar
  30. Magnan, A. (1993). Le traitement de l'information ordinale: une étape nécessaire de l'apprentissage de la lecture [Processing ordinal information: a necessary phase in learning to read], l'Année Psychologique [The Psychological Year] 93: 185-200.Google Scholar
  31. Mann, V.A. (1986). Phonological awareness: the role of reading experience, Cognition 24: 65-92.Google Scholar
  32. Marsh, G., Friedman, M., Welsh, V. &; Desberg, P. (1981). A cognitive developmental theory of reading acquisition. In: G.E. MacKinnon &; T.G. Waller (eds.), Reading Research: Advances in theory and practice (vol. 3, pp. 19-221). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  33. Mason, J. (1980). When do children begin to read: an exploration of four-year-olds'letters and word reading competencies, Reading Research Quarterly 15: 203-227.Google Scholar
  34. Masonheimer, P.E., Drum, P.A. &; Ehri, L.C. (1984). Does environmental print identification lead children into word reading? Reading Behavior 16: 257-271.Google Scholar
  35. Morais, J. (1993). Compréhension/décodage et acquisition de la lecture [ Comprehension/ deciphering and reading acquisition]. In: J-P. Jaffré, L. Sprenger-Charolles &; M. Fayol (eds.), Lecture-écriture: acquisition [Reading-writing: acquisition] (pp. 10-21). Paris: Nathan.Google Scholar
  36. Morton, J. (1989). An information-processing account of reading acquisition. In: A.M. Galaburda (ed.), From reading to neurons (pp. 43-66). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  37. Richard, J-F. (1995). Les activités mentales: comprendre, raisonner, trouver des solutions [Mental activities: understanding, reasoning, finding solutions], 2nd edition. Paris: Masson &; A. Colin.Google Scholar
  38. Sebastian-Galles, N. &; Parreño Vacchiano, A. (1995). The development of analogical reading in Spanish, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7: 23-38.Google Scholar
  39. Seymour, P.H.K. &; Elder, L. (1986). Beginning reading without phonology, Cognitive Neuropsychology 3: 1-37.Google Scholar
  40. Snowling, M.J. &; Hulme, C. (1992). Déficits phonologiques et troubles de la lecture: une étude de cas [Phonological disability and reading difficulties: a case study]. In: P. Lecocq (ed.), La lecture: processus, apprentissage, troubles [Reading: processes, learning, troubles] (pp. 253-264). Lille: Presses Universitaires de Lille.Google Scholar
  41. Sprenger-Charolles, L. (1994). L'acquisiton de la lecture en français: étude longitudinale de la première à la seconde année du primaire [Reading acquisition in French: a longitudinal study from the first to the second grade], L'Année Psychologique [The Psychological Year] 94: 553-574.Google Scholar
  42. Sprenger-Charolles, L. &; Casalis, S. (1995). Reading and spelling acquisition in French first graders: longitudinal evidence, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7: 39-63.Google Scholar
  43. Sprenger-Charolles, L. &; Casalis, S. (1996). Lire. Lecture et écriture: acquisition et troubles du développement [Reading and writing: acquisition and developmental difficulties]. Paris: P.U.F.Google Scholar
  44. Sprenger-Charolles, L. &; Bonnet, P. (1996). New doubts on the importance of the logographic stage, Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, Current Psychology of Cognition 15: 173-208.Google Scholar
  45. Stuart, M. &; Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages, Cognition 30: 139-181.Google Scholar
  46. Vallieres, N. (1997). L'apprentissage de la lecture par des adultes analphabètes [Reading acquisition in illiterate adults], Master's thesis in psychology, Université de Provence.Google Scholar
  47. Wimmer, H. &; Hummer, P. (1990). How German-speaking first graders read and spell: doubts on the importance of the logographic stage, Applied Psycholinguistics 11: 349-368.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mireille Bastien-Toniazzo
    • 1
  • Sandrine Jullien
    • 2
  1. 1.U.F.R. Psychologie, Sciences de l'EducationUniversité de ProvenceAix-en-ProvenceFrance
  2. 2.Université de ProvenceAix-en-ProvenceFrance

Personalised recommendations