Advertisement

Sex Roles

, Volume 42, Issue 11–12, pp 1007–1025 | Cite as

Gender-Role Cognition in Three-Year-Old Boys and Girls

  • Marion O'Brien
  • Vicki Peyton
  • Rashmita Mistry
  • Ludmila Hruda
  • Anne Jacobs
  • Yvonne Caldera
  • Aletha Huston
  • Carolyn Roy
Article

Abstract

Although the multidimensionality of gender roles has been well established, few researchers have investigated male and female roles separately. Because of the substantial differences in the ways male and female roles are portrayed in our culture, boys and girls may think and learn about these roles differently. The male role is more clearly defined, more highly valued, and more salient than the female role; thus, children's cognitions about these two roles may be expected to differ. The present study addressed the question of whether there is sex-typical variation in gender labeling, gender-role knowledge, and schematicity. Participants were 120 families; 15% were from minority ethnic groups, and 17% were single-parent families; 25% of the parents had a high school education or less. Results indicated that at 36 months of age, boys were less able to label gender and less knowledgeable about gender roles than were girls. Boys' knew more about male stereotypes than female stereotypes, whereas girls knew considerably more than boys about the female role and as much as boys about the male role. Boys and girls were found to be similar in gender schematicity. Traditionality of parental attitudes regarding child-rearing and maternal employment were not strongly related to children's gender cognition.

Keywords

High School Ethnic Group Social Psychology Gender Role School Education 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.Google Scholar
  2. Bigler, R. S. (1997). Conceptual and methodological issues in the measurement of children's sex typing. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 53–69.Google Scholar
  3. Biller, H. B. (1993). Fathers and families: Paternal factors in child development. Westport, CT: Auburn House.Google Scholar
  4. Blakemore, J., LaRue, A., & Olejnik, A. (1979). Sex-appropriate toy preference and the ability to conceptualize toys as sex-role related. Developmental Psychology, 15, 339–340.Google Scholar
  5. Block, J. H. (1976). Issues, problems, and pitfalls in assessing sex differences: A critical review of the psychology of sex differences. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 22, 283–308.Google Scholar
  6. Bradley, B. S., & Gobbart, S. K. (1989). Determinants of gender-typed play in toddlers. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150, 453–455.Google Scholar
  7. Caldera, Y. M., Huston, A. C., & O'Brien, M. (1989). Social interactions and play patterns of parents and toddlers with feminine, masculine, and neutral toys. Child Development, 60, 70–76.Google Scholar
  8. Calvert, S. L., & Huston, A. C. (1987). Television and children's gender schemata. In L. S. Liben and M. L. Signorella (Eds.), Children's gender schemata (pp. 75–88). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  9. Carter, D. B., & Levy, G. D. (1988). Cognitive aspects of early sex-role development: The influence of gender schemas on preschoolers' memories and preferences for sex-typed toys and activities. Child Development, 59, 782–792.Google Scholar
  10. Carter, D. B., & Levy, G. D. (1991). Gender schemas and the salience of gender: Individual differences in nonreversal discrimination learning. Sex Roles, 25, 555–567.Google Scholar
  11. Citron, C. C., Serbin, L. A., & Connor, J. M. (1979, March). Children's observational learning of same and opposite sex model behaviors. Paper presented at the conference of the Association for Women in Psychology, Dallas, Texas.Google Scholar
  12. Dambrot, F. H., Reep, D. C., & Bell, D. (1988). Television sex roles in the 1980s: Do viewers' ses and sex role orientation change the picture? Sex Roles, 19, 387–401.Google Scholar
  13. Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women's achievement-related decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 135–172.Google Scholar
  14. Edelbrock, C., & Sugawara, A. I. (1978). Acquisition of sex-typed preferences in preschoolaged children. Developmental Psychology, 14, 614–623.Google Scholar
  15. Fagot, B. I., & Leinbach, M. D. (1989). The young child's gender schema: Environmental input, internal organization. Child Development, 60, 663–672.Google Scholar
  16. Fagot, B. I., & Leinbach, M. D. (1993). Gender-role discrimination in young children: From discrimination to labeling. Developmental Review, 13, 205–224.Google Scholar
  17. Fagot, B. I., & Leinbach, M. D. (1995). Gender knowledge in egalitarian and traditional families. Sex Roles, 32, 513–526.Google Scholar
  18. Fagot, B. I., Leinbach, M. D., & Hagan, R. (1986). Gender labeling and the adoption of sextyped behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 22, 440–443.Google Scholar
  19. Fagot, B. I., Leinbach, M. D., & O'Boyle, C. (1992). Gender labeling, gender stereotyping, and parenting behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 28, 225–230.Google Scholar
  20. Garrett, C. S., Ein, P. L., & Tremaine, L. (1977). The development of gender stereotyping of adult occupations in elementary school children. Child Development, 48, 507–512.Google Scholar
  21. Greenberger, E., Goldberg, W. A., Crawford, T. J., & Granger, J. (1988). Beliefs about consequences of maternal employment for children. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12, 35–59.Google Scholar
  22. Huston, A. C. (1983). Sex-typing. In E. M. Hetherington and P. H. Mussen (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 387–467). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  23. Huston, A. C. (1985). The development of sex typing: Themes from recent research. Developmental Review, 5, 1–17.Google Scholar
  24. Ignico, A. A. (1990). The influence of gender-role perception on activity preferences of children. Play and Culture, 3, 302–310.Google Scholar
  25. Jacobs, J. E., & Eccles, J. S. (1992). The impact of mothers' gender-role stereotypic beliefs on mothers' and children's ability perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 932–944.Google Scholar
  26. Katz, P. A. (1985). Gender identity development and consequences. In R. D. Ashmore and F. K. DelBoca (Eds.), The social psychology of female-male relations (pp. 21–67). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  27. Katz, P. A. (1987). Variations in family constellation: Effects on gender schemata. In L. S. Liben and M. L. Signorella (Eds.), Children's gender schemata (pp. 39–56). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  28. Katz, P. A., & Boswell, S. (1986). Flexibility and traditionality in children's gender roles. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 112, 105–145.Google Scholar
  29. Katz, P. A., & Ksansnak, K. R. (1994). Developmental aspects of gender role flexibility and traditionality in middle childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30, 272–282.Google Scholar
  30. Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children's sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82–173). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Leinbach, M. D., & Fagot, B. I. (1986). Acquisition of gender labels: A test for toddlers. Sex Roles, 15, 655–666.Google Scholar
  32. Leinbach, M. D., Hort, B. E., & Fagot, B. I. (1997). Bears are for boys: Metaphorical associations in young children's gender stereotypes. Cognitive Development, 12, 107–130.Google Scholar
  33. Levy, G. D. (1989a). Developmental and individual differences in preschoolers' recognition memories: The influences of gender schematization and verbal labeling of information. Sex Roles, 21, 305–324.Google Scholar
  34. Levy, G. D. (1989b). Relations among aspects of children's social environments, gender schematization, gender role knowledge, and flexibility. Sex Roles, 21, 803–823.Google Scholar
  35. Levy, G. D. (1994). Aspects of preschoolers' comprehension of indoor and outdoor gendertyped toys. Sex Roles, 30, 391–405.Google Scholar
  36. Levy, G. D., & Carter, D. B. (1989). Gender-schema, gender constancy, and gender-role knowledge: The role of cognitive factors in preschoolers' gender-role stereotype attributions. Developmental Psychology, 25, 444–449.Google Scholar
  37. Liben, L. S., & Signorella, M. (Eds.) (1987). Children's gender schemata: New directions for child development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  38. Lloyd, B., Duveen, G., & Smith, C. (1988). Social representations of gender and young children's play: A replication. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 83–88.Google Scholar
  39. Lobel, T. E., & Menashri, J. (1993). Relations of conceptions of gender-role transgressions and gender constancy to gender-typed toy preferences. Developmental Psychology, 29, 150–155.Google Scholar
  40. Martin, C. L. (1993). New directions for investigating children's gender knowledge. Developmental Review, 13, 184–204.Google Scholar
  41. Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1134.Google Scholar
  42. Martin, C. L., & Little, J. K. (1990). The relation of gender understanding to children's sextyped preferences and gender stereotypes. Child Development, 61, 1427–1439.Google Scholar
  43. Martin, C. L., Wood, C. H., & Little, J. K. (1990). The development of gender stereotype components. Child Development, 61, 1891–1904.Google Scholar
  44. O'Brien, M. (1992). Gender identity and sex roles. In V. B. VanHasselt & M. Herson (Eds.), Handbook of social development: A lifespan perspective (pp. 325–345). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  45. O'Brien, M., & Huston, A. C. (1985). Development of sex-typed play behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 21, 866–871.Google Scholar
  46. Pellett, T. L., & Harrison, J. M. (1992). Children's perception of the gender appropriateness of physical activities: A further analysis. Play and Culture, 5, 305–313.Google Scholar
  47. Perry, D. G., White, A. J., & Perry, L. C. (1984). Does early sex typing result from children's attempts to match their behavior to sex role stereotypes? Child Development, 55, 2114–2121.Google Scholar
  48. Roopnarine, J. L. (1984). Sex-typed socialization in mixed-age preschool classrooms. Child Development, 55, 1078–1084.Google Scholar
  49. Schaefer, E. S., & Edgerton, M. (1985). Parent and child correlates of parental modernity. In I. E. Sigel (Ed.), Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children (pp. 287–318). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  50. Serbin, L. A., & Connor, J. M. (1979). Sex-typing of children's play preferences and patterns of cognitive performance. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 134, 315–316.Google Scholar
  51. Serbin, L. A., & Sprafkin, C. (1986). The salience of gender and the process of sex typing in three-to seven-year-old children. Child Development, 57, 1188–1199.Google Scholar
  52. Siegel, M. (1987). Are sons and daughters treated more differently by fathers than by mothers? Developmental Review, 7, 183–209.Google Scholar
  53. Signorella, M. L. (1987). Gender schemata: Individual differences and context effects. In L. S. Liben and M. L. Signorella (Eds.), Children's gender schemata (pp. 23–37). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  54. Signorella, M. L., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1993). Developmental differences in children's gender schemata about others: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 13, 147–183.Google Scholar
  55. Slaby, R. G., & Frey, K. S. (1975). Development of gender constancy and selective attention to same-sex models. Child Development, 46, 849–856.Google Scholar
  56. Stangor, C., & Ruble, D. N. (1987). Development of gender role knowledge and gender constancy. In L. S. Liben, and M. L. Signorella (Eds.), Children's gender schemata (pp. 5–22). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  57. Stangor, C., & Ruble, D. N. (1989). Differential influences of gender schemata and gender constancy on children's information processing and behavior. Social Cognition, 7, 353–372.Google Scholar
  58. Tognoli, J., Pullen, J., & Lieber, J. (1994). The privilege of place: Domestic and work locations of characters in children's books. Children's Environments, 11, 272–280.Google Scholar
  59. Urberg, K. A. (1982). The development of the concepts of masculinity and femininity in young children. Sex Roles, 8, 659–668.Google Scholar
  60. Weinraub, M., Clements, L. P., Sockloff, A., Ethridge, T., Gracely, E., & Myers, B. (1984). The development of sex role stereotypes in the third year: Relationships to gender labeling, gender identity, sex typed toy preferences. Child Development, 55, 1493–1503.Google Scholar
  61. Weisner, T. S., & Wilson-Mitchell, J. E. (1990). Nonconventional family life-styles and sex typing in six-year-olds. Child Development, 61, 1915–1933.Google Scholar
  62. Williams, J. E., Bennett, S. M., & Best, D. L. (1973). Awareness and expression of sex stereotypes in young children. Developmental Psychology, 11, 635–642.Google Scholar
  63. Williams, K., Goodman, M., & Green, R. (1985). Parent-child factors in gender role socialization in girls. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, 720–731.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marion O'Brien
    • 1
  • Vicki Peyton
    • 2
  • Rashmita Mistry
    • 3
  • Ludmila Hruda
    • 4
  • Anne Jacobs
    • 2
  • Yvonne Caldera
    • 5
  • Aletha Huston
    • 6
  • Carolyn Roy
    • 2
  1. 1.University of KansasUSA
  2. 2.University of KansasUSA
  3. 3.University of North Carolina Center for Developmental ScienceUSA
  4. 4.University of MichiganAnn Arbor
  5. 5.Texas Tech UniversityLubbock
  6. 6.University of Texas at AustinAustin

Personalised recommendations