Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 30, Issue 4, pp 295–304 | Cite as

Episodic Memory and Remembering in Adults with Asperger Syndrome

  • Dermot M. Bowler
  • John M. Gardiner
  • Sarah J. Grice

Abstract

A group of adults with Asperger syndrome and an IQ-matched control group were compared in remember versus know recognition memory. Word frequency was also manipulated. Both groups showed superior recognition for low-frequency compared with high-frequency words, and in both groups this word frequency effect occurred in remembering, not in knowing. Nor did overall recognition differ between the two groups. However, recognition in the Asperger group was associated with less remembering, and more knowing, than in the control group. Since remembering reflects autonoetic consciousness, which is the hallmark of an episodic memory system, these results show that episodic memory is moderately impaired in individuals with Asperger syndrome even when overall recognition performance is not.

Asperger syndrome remembering memory knowing memory 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Asperger, H. (1944). Die “autistische Psychopathen” im Kindesalter. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136.Google Scholar
  2. Asperger, H. (1991). Autistic psychopathy in childhood (V. Frith, Trans.). In U. Frith (Ed.), Autism and Asperger syndrome. Cambridge, UK: CUP. (Original work published 1944).Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind'? Cognition, 21, 37–46.Google Scholar
  4. Barth, C., Fein, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1995). Delayed match-to-sample performance in autistic children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 11, 53–69.Google Scholar
  5. Benetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory function in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835.Google Scholar
  6. Bishop, D. (1993). Autism, executive functions and Theory of Mind: A neuropsychological perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 279–294.Google Scholar
  7. Blaxton, T. A., & Theodore, W. H. (1997). The role of the temporal lobes in recognizing visuospatial materials: Remembering versus knowing. Brain and Cognition, 35, 5–25.Google Scholar
  8. Boucher, J. (1981). Immediate free recall in early childhood autism: Another point of similarity with the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 72, 211–215.Google Scholar
  9. Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1989). Memory impairments and communication in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 99–122.Google Scholar
  10. Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. (1976). Memory deficits in early infantile autism: Some similarities to the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 73–87.Google Scholar
  11. Bowler, D. M. (1992) 'Theory of Mind' in Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 877–893.Google Scholar
  12. Bowler, D. M. (1997). Reaction times to mental state and non-mental state questions in false belief tasks by high-functioning individuals with autism. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 6, 160–165.Google Scholar
  13. Bowler, D. M., Matthews, N. J., & Gardiner, J. M. (1997). Asperger's syndrome and memory: Similarity to autism but not amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35, 65–70.Google Scholar
  14. Burack, J. A., Enns, J. T., Stauder, J. E. A., Mottron, L., & Randolph, B. (1997). Attention and autism: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 226–247). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  15. Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 497–508.Google Scholar
  16. Dalla Barba, G. (1997). Recognition memory and recollective experience in Alzheimer's disease. Memory, 5, 657–672.Google Scholar
  17. Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism. Archives of Neurology, 35, 777–786.Google Scholar
  18. Farrant, A., Blades, M., & Boucher, J. (1998). Source monitoring by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 43–50.Google Scholar
  19. Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1990). Recollective experience in word and nonword recognition. Memory & Cognition, 18, 23–30.Google Scholar
  20. Gardiner, J. M, Kaminska, Z., Dixon, M., & Java, R. I. (1996). Repetition of previously novel melodies sometimes increases both remember and know responses in recognition memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 366–371.Google Scholar
  21. Gardiner, J. M., & Radomski, E. (1999). Awareness of recognition memory for Polish and English folk songs in Polish and English folk. Memory, 1, 461–470.Google Scholar
  22. Gardiner, J. M., Ramponi, C., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (1998). Experiences of remembering, knowing, and guessing. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 1–26.Google Scholar
  23. Gardiner, J. M., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2000). Remembering and knowing. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Handbook of memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Gardiner, J. M., Richardson-Klavehn, A., & Ramponi, C. (1997). On reporting recollective experiences and “direct access to memory systems.” Psychological Science, 8, 391–394.Google Scholar
  25. Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, I. C. (1989). Asperger syndrome-some epidemiological considerations: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 631–638.Google Scholar
  26. Happé, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the Theory of Mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843–855.Google Scholar
  27. Happé, F., & Frith, U. (1996). The neuropsychology of autism. Brain, 119, 1377–1400.Google Scholar
  28. Hermelin, B., & O'Conor, N. (1967). Remembering of words by psychotic and subnormal children. British Journal of Psychology, 58, 213–218.Google Scholar
  29. Huron, C., Danion, J.-M., Giacomoni, F., Grange, D., Robert, P., & Rizzo, L. (1995). Impairment of recognition memory with, but not without, conscious recollection in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1737–1742.Google Scholar
  30. Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsey, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28.Google Scholar
  31. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.Google Scholar
  32. Kinoshita, S. (1995). The word frequency effect in recognition memory versus repetition priming. Memory & Cognition, 23, 569–580.Google Scholar
  33. Kucera, H., & Francis. W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day american english. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1995). Remembering and knowing: Two different expressions of declarative memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 699–710.Google Scholar
  35. Mantyla, T. (1993). Knowing but not remembering: Adult age differences in recollective experience. Memory & Cognition, 21, 379–388.Google Scholar
  36. Mantyla, T. (1997). Recollections of faces: Remembering differences and knowing similarities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1203–1216.Google Scholar
  37. Minshew, N., Goldstein, G., Muenz, L. R., & Payton, J. (1992). Neuropsychological functioning in nonmentally retarded autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 749–761.Google Scholar
  38. Minshew, N., Goldstein, G., Taylor, H., & Siegel, D. (1994). Academic achievement in high-functioning autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 261–270.Google Scholar
  39. Nyberg, L., & Tulving, E. (1996). Classifying human long term memory: Evidence from converging dissociations. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 8, 163–183.Google Scholar
  40. O'Connor, N., & Hermelin, B. (1967). Auditory and visual memory in autistic and normal children. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 11, 126–131.Google Scholar
  41. Parkin, A. J., & Walter, B. (1992). Recollective experience, normal aging, and frontal dysfunction. Psychology and Aging, 7, 290–298.Google Scholar
  42. Pascualvaca, D. M., Fantie, B. D., Papageorgiou, M., & Mirsky, A. F. (1998). Attentional capacities in children with autism: Is there a general deficit in shifting focus? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 467–478.Google Scholar
  43. Pennington, B., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.Google Scholar
  44. Perfect, T. J., & Dasgupta, Z. R. R. (1997). What underlies the deficit in reported recollective experience in old age? Memory & Cognition, 25, 849–858.Google Scholar
  45. Perfect, T. J., Williams, R. B., & Anderton-Brown, C. (1995). Age differences in reported recollective experience are due to encoding effects, not response bias. Memory, 3, 169–186.Google Scholar
  46. Perner, J. (1990). Experiential awareness and children's episodic memory. In W. Schneider & F. E. Weinert (Eds.), Interactions among aptitudes, strategies and knowledge in cognitive performance. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  47. Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (1995). Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: Developmental evidence and a theory of childhood amnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 516–548.Google Scholar
  48. Rajaram, S. (1996) Perceptual effects on remembering: Recollective processes in picture-recognition memory. Journal Of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory And Cognition, 22, 365–377.Google Scholar
  49. Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988) Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 201–221.Google Scholar
  50. Schacter, D. L., Verfaillie, M., & Anes, M. D. (1997). Illusory memories in amnesic patients: Conceptual and perceptual false recognition. Neuropsychology, 11, 331–342.Google Scholar
  51. Strack, F., & Forster, J. (1995). Reporting recollective experiences: Direct access to memory systems? Psychological Science, 6, 352–358.Google Scholar
  52. Szatmari, P., Bremner, R., & Nagy, J. (1989). Asperger's syndrome: A review of clinical features. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34, 554–560.Google Scholar
  53. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 417–430.Google Scholar
  54. Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychologist, 26, 1–12.Google Scholar
  56. Waterhouse, L., Fein, D., & Modahl, C. (1996). Neurofunctional mechanisms in autism. Psychological Review, 103, 457–489.Google Scholar
  57. Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Towards a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 331–354.Google Scholar
  58. World Health Organization. (1992). Tenth revision of the international classification of disease. Geneva: Author.Google Scholar
  59. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.Google Scholar
  60. Wing, L. (1981). Asperger's syndrome: A clinical account. Psychological Medicine, 11, 115–129.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Dermot M. Bowler
    • 1
  • John M. Gardiner
    • 2
  • Sarah J. Grice
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyCity UniversityLondonUnited Kingdom
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyCity UniversityLondonUnited Kingdom

Personalised recommendations