Advertisement

Synthese

, Volume 122, Issue 1–2, pp 3–28 | Cite as

How the Social Environment Shaped the Evolution of Mind

  • Denise Dellarosa Cummins
Article

Abstract

Dominance hierarchies are ubiquitous in the societies of human and non-human animals. Evidence from comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychological investigations is presented that show how social dominance hierarchies shaped the evolution of the human mind, and hence, human social institutions. It is argued that the pressures that arise from living in hierarchical social groups laid a foundation of fundamental concepts and cognitive strategies that are crucial to surviving in social dominance hierarchies. These include recognizing and reasoning transitively about dominance relations, fast-track learning of social norms (permissions, prohibitions, and obligations), detecting violations of social norms (cheating), monitoring reciprocal obligations, and reading the intentions of others.

Keywords

Social Group Social Norm Social Environment Social Institution Dominance Relation 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Altmann, J., S. C. Alberts, S. A. Haines, J. Dubach, P. Muruth, T. Coote, E. Geffen, D. J. Cheesman, R. A. Mututua, S.N. Saiyalel, R. K. Wayne, R. C. Lacy, and M. W. Bruford: 1996, 'Behavior Predicts Genetic Structure in a Wild Primate Group', Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93, 5795–801.Google Scholar
  2. Anew, A.: 1996, 'Innateness and Canalization', Philosophy of Science 63, S19–S27.Google Scholar
  3. Aruguete, M.: 1994, 'Cognition, Tradition, and the Explanation of Social Behavior in Non-Human Primates', [Review of Social Processes and Mental Abilities in Non-Human Primates]. American Journal of Primatology 33, 71–4.Google Scholar
  4. Axelrod, R., and W. D. Hamilton: 1981, 'The Evolution of Cooperation', Science 211, 1390–6.Google Scholar
  5. Bartsch, K. and H. M. Wellman: 1989, 'Young Children's Attribution of Action to Beliefs and Desires', Child Development 60, 946–64.Google Scholar
  6. Bentham, J.: 1789, The Principles of Morals and Legislation, Prometheus Books, Buffalo, N.Y.Google Scholar
  7. Bertram, B. C. R.: 1976, 'Kin Selection in Lions and Evolution', in P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde (eds), Growing Points in Ethology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 281–302.Google Scholar
  8. Boehm, C.: 1992, 'Segmentary “Warfare” and theManagement of Conflict: Comparison of East African Chimpanzees and Patrilineal-Patrilocal Humans', in A. Harcourt and F. B. M. de Waal (eds), Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals, pp. 137–75, Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  9. Boesch, C.: 1991, 'Teaching in Wild Chimpanzees', Animal Behavior 41, 530–2.Google Scholar
  10. Bygott, J. D., B. C. R. Bertram, and J. P. Hanby: 1979, 'Male Lions in Large Coalitions Gain Reproductive Advantage', Nature 282, 839–41.Google Scholar
  11. Byrne, R.: 1995, The Thinking Ape: Evolutionary Origins Of Intelligence, Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  12. Bussey, K.: 1992, 'Children's Lying and Truthfulness: Implications for Children's Testimony', in S. J. Ceci, M. De Simone Leichtman, and M. Putnick (eds), Cognitive and Social Factors in Early Deception, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 89–110.Google Scholar
  13. Caro, T. M. and M. D. Hauser: 1992, 'Is There Teaching in Non-Human Animals?', Quarterly Review of Biology 67, 151–74.Google Scholar
  14. Chapais, B.: 1988, 'Rank Maintenance in Female Japanese Macaques: Experimental Evidence For Social Dependency', iBehavior 104, 41–59.Google Scholar
  15. Chapais, B.: 1992, 'Role of Alliances in the Social Inheritance of Rank Among Female Primates', in A. Harcourt and F. B. M De Waal (eds), Cooperation in Contests in Animals and Humans, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 29–60.Google Scholar
  16. Cheney, D. L.: 1983, 'Extra-Familial Alliances Among Vervet Monkeys', in R. A. Hinde (ed.), Primate Social Relationships, Blackwell, Oxford.Google Scholar
  17. Cheney, D. L. and R. M. Seyfarth: 1990, How Monkeys See the World, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  18. Cheng, P. W. and K. J. Holyoak: 1985, 'Pragmatic Reasoning Schemas', Cognitive Psychology 17, 391–416.Google Scholar
  19. Cheng, P. W. and K. J. Holyoak: 1989, 'On the Natural Selection of Reasoning Theories', Cognition 33, 285–313.Google Scholar
  20. Clutton-Brock, T. H.: 1988, 'Reproductive Success', in T. H. Clutton-Brock (ed.), Reproductive Success, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  21. Clutton-Brock, T. H. and P. H. Harvey: 1976, 'Evolutionary Rules and Primate Societies', in P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde (eds), Growing Points in Ethology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 195–238.Google Scholar
  22. Cosmides, L.: 1989, 'The Logic of Social Exchange: Has Natural Selection Shaped How Humans Reason? Studies With the Wason Selection Task', Cognition 31, 187–276.Google Scholar
  23. Cosmides, L. and J. Tooby: 1992, 'Cognitive Adaptations for Social Exchange', in J. Barkow, L. Cosmides and J. Tooby (eds), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  24. Cosmides, L. and J. Tooby: 1994, 'Origins of Domain Specificity: The Evolution of Functional Organization', in L. A. Hirshfeld and S. Gelman (eds), Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, pp. 85–116, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  25. Cummins, D. D.: 1996(a), 'Evidence of Deontic Reasoning in 3-and 4-Year-olds', Memory and Cognition 24, 823–9.Google Scholar
  26. Cummins, D. D.: 1996(b), 'Evidence for the Innateness of Deontic Reasoning', Mind and Language 11, 160–90.Google Scholar
  27. Cummins, D. D.: 1996(c), 'Dominance Hierarchies and the Evolution of Human Reasoning', Minds and Machines 6, 463–80.Google Scholar
  28. Cummins, D. D.: 1997, 'Rationality: Biological, Psychology, and Normative Theories', Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive (Current Psychology of Cognition 16, 78–86.Google Scholar
  29. Cummins, D. D.: 1998(a), 'Social Norms and Other Minds: The Evolutionary Roots of Higher Cognition', in D. D. Cummins and C. A. Allen (eds), The Evolution of Mind, Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 30–50.Google Scholar
  30. Cummins, D. D.: 1998(b), 'Can Humans Form Hierarchically Embedded Mental Representations?' Commentary on R. W. Byrne and A. E. Russon 'Learning By Imitation: A Hierarchical Approach', Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21, 687–8.Google Scholar
  31. Cummins, D. D.: 1999, 'Cheater Detection is Modified by Social Rank', Evolution and Human Behavior 20, 229–48.Google Scholar
  32. Cummins, D. D.: in press, 'Adaptive Cognitive Mechanisms: Reasoning about Social Norms and Other Minds', in R. Elio (ed.), Commonsense, Reasoning, and Rationality: Vancouver Studies in Cognitive Science, Vol. 11, Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  33. Cummins, D. D. and R. C. Cummins: in press, 'Biological Preparedness and Evolutionary Explanation', Cognition.Google Scholar
  34. Datta, S. B.: 1983(a), 'Relative Power and the Acquisition of Rank', in R. A. Hinde (ed.), Primate Social Relationships, Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  35. Datta, S. B.: 1983(b), 'Relative Power and the Maintenance of Rank', in R. A. Hinde (ed.), Primate Social Relationships, Blackwell, Oxford.Google Scholar
  36. Dewsbury, D. A.: 1982, 'Dominance Rank, Copulatory Behavior and Differential Reproduction', Quarterly Review of Biology 57, 135–59.Google Scholar
  37. Duhem, P.: 1954, The Aim and Structure of Physical Theory, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ (Original work published in 1914.)Google Scholar
  38. Earman, J.: 1992, Bayes or Bust?, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  39. Ellis, L.: 1995, 'Dominance and Reproductive Success Among Nonhuman Animals: A Cross-Species Comparison', Ethology and Sociobiology 16, 257–333.Google Scholar
  40. Fedigan, L.: 1983, 'Dominance and Reproductive Success in Primates', Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 26, 91–129.Google Scholar
  41. Flavell, J. H., E. R. Flavell, G. L. Green, and L. J. Moses: 1990, 'Young Children”s Understanding of Fact Beliefs Versus Value Beliefs', Child Development 61, 915–28.Google Scholar
  42. Frankel, D. G. and T. Arbel: 1980, 'Group Formation by Two-Year-Olds', International Journal of Behavioral Development 3, 287–98.Google Scholar
  43. Frankena, W. K.: 1963, Ethics, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.Google Scholar
  44. Gagneux, P., D. S. Woodruff, and C. Boesch: 1997, 'Furtive Mating in Female Chimpanzees', Nature 387, 358–69.Google Scholar
  45. Goodall, J.: 1986, The Chimpanzees of Gombe, Belknap Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  46. Gopnik, A.: 1993, 'How We Know Our Minds: The Illusion of First-Person Knowledge of Intentionality', Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16, 1–15.Google Scholar
  47. Griggs, R. A. and J. R. Cox: 1982, 'The Elusive Thematic Materials Effect in Wason's Selection Task', British Journal of Psychology 73, 407–20.Google Scholar
  48. Hall, K. R. L.: 1964, 'Aggression in Monkey and Ape Societies', in J. Carthy and F. Ebling (eds), The Natural History of Aggression, Academic Press, London, pp. 51–64.Google Scholar
  49. Harcourt, A. H.: 1988, 'Alliances in Contests and Social Intelligence', in R. W. Byrne and A. Whiten (eds), Machiavellian Intelligence, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 131–52.Google Scholar
  50. Harcourt, A. H. and K. J. Stewart: 1987, 'The Influence of Help in Contests on Dominance Rank in Primates: Hints From Gorillas', Animal Behaviour 35, 182–90.Google Scholar
  51. Harcourt, A. H. and F. B. M. de Waal (eds), 1992, Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals, Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  52. Harkness, S., C. Pope Edwards, and C. M. Super: 1981, 'Social Roles and Moral Reasoning: A Case Study in a Rural African Community', Developmental Psychology 17, 595–603.Google Scholar
  53. Harris, P. L.. and M. Nuñez: 1996, 'Understanding of Permission Rules by Preschool Children', Child Development 67, 1572–91.Google Scholar
  54. Haugaard, J. and N. D. Reppucci: 1992, 'Children and the Truth', in S. J. Ceci, M. De Simone Leichtman and M. Putnick (eds), Cognitive and Social Factors in Early Deception, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 29–46.Google Scholar
  55. Hausfater, G.: 1975, 'Dominance and Reproduction in Baboons (Papio Cynocephalus): A Quantitative Analysis', Contributions in Primatology 7, 1–150.Google Scholar
  56. Haviland, J. M. and M. Lelivica: 1987, 'The Induced Affect Response: 10 Week Old Infants' Responses to Three Emotional Expressions', Developmental Psychology 23, 97–104.Google Scholar
  57. Hilpinen, R.: 1971, Deontic Logic: Introductory and Systematic Readings, Reidel/Kluwer, Boston.Google Scholar
  58. Hilpinen, R.: 1981, New Studies in Deontic Logic, Reidel/Kluwer, Boston.Google Scholar
  59. Hokanson, J. E.: 1961, 'The Effect of Frustration and Anxiety on Overt Aggression', Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 62, 346–51.Google Scholar
  60. Hokanson, J. E. and S. Shetler: 1961, 'The Effect of Overt Aggression on Physiological Arousal', Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 63, 446–8.Google Scholar
  61. Hold-Cavell, B. C. and D. Borsutzky: 1986, 'Longitudinal Study of a Group of Preschool Children', Ethology and Sociobiology 7, 39–56.Google Scholar
  62. Hollos, M., P. E. Leis and E. Turiel: 1986, 'Social Reasoning in Ijo Children and Adolescents in Nigerian Communities', Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 17, 352–74.Google Scholar
  63. Holyoak, K. J. and P. W. Cheng: 1995, 'Pragmatic Reasoning with a Point of View', 1, 289–313.Google Scholar
  64. Horwich, P.: 1982, Probability and Evidence, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  65. Jenkins, J. and J. W. Astington: 1996, 'Cognitive Factors and Family Structure Associated with Theory of Mind Development in Young Children', Developmental Psychology 32, 70–8.Google Scholar
  66. Johnson-Laird, P. N., P. Legrenzi and M. S. Legrenzi: 1972, 'Reasoning and a Sense of Reality', British Journal of Psychology 63, 395–400.Google Scholar
  67. Keating, C. F. and K. R. Heltman: 1994, 'Dominance and Deception in Children and Adults: Are Leaders the Best Misleaders?', Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 54, 312–21.Google Scholar
  68. Kosslyn, S. M. and O. Koenig: 1995, Wet Mind: The New Cognitive Neuroscience, Free Press, New York.Google Scholar
  69. Krebs, J. R. and R. Dawkins: 1984, 'Animal Signals: Mind-Reading and Manipulation', in J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies (eds), Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, 2nd ed., Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 380–402.Google Scholar
  70. La Freniere, P. and W. R. Charlesworth: 1983, 'Dominance, Attention, and Affiliation in a Preschool Group: A Nine-Month Longitudinal Study', Ethology and Sociobiology 4, 55–67.Google Scholar
  71. Lakatos, I.: 1970, 'Falsification and the Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes', in I. Lakatos and A. Musgrave (eds), Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 91–196.Google Scholar
  72. Leslie, A. M.: 1994, 'ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core Architecture and Domain Specificity', in L. A. Hirshfeld and S. A. Gelman (eds), Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 119–48.Google Scholar
  73. Lewis, C., N. Freeman, C. Kyriakidou, K. Maridaki-Kassotaki and D. Berridge: 1996, 'Social Influences on False Belief Access: Specific Contagion or General Apprenticeship?', Child Development 67, 2930–47.Google Scholar
  74. Manktelow, K. I. and D. E. Over: 1991, 'Social Roles and Utilities in Reasoning With Deontic Conditionals', Cognition 39, 85–105.Google Scholar
  75. Manktelow, K. I., and D. E. Over: 1995, 'Deontic Reasoning', in S. E. Newstead and J. St. B. Evans (eds), Perspectives On Thinking and Reasoning, Erlbaum, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.Google Scholar
  76. McCann, T. S.: 1981, 'Aggression and Sexual Activity of Male Southern Elephant Seals (Mirounga Leonina)', Journal of Zoology 195, 295–310.Google Scholar
  77. McGuffin, P. and J. Scourfield: 1997, 'A Father's Imprint on His Daughter's Thinking', Nature 387, 652–3.Google Scholar
  78. McKenzie, J. A. and K. O'Farrell: 1993, 'Modification of Developmental Instability and Fitness: Malathion-Resistance in the Australian Sheep Blowfly', Genetica 89, 67–76.Google Scholar
  79. Mealey, L., C. Daood and M. Krage: 1996, 'Enhanced Memory for Faces of Cheaters', Ethology and Sociobiology 17, 119–28.Google Scholar
  80. Meltzoff, A. N.: 1995, 'Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-enactment of Intended Acts by 18-month-old Children', Developmental Psychology 31, 838–50.Google Scholar
  81. Menzel, E. W.: 1974, 'A Group of Chimpanzees in a 1-Acre Field: Leadership and Communication', in A. M. Schrier and F. Stollnitz (eds), Behavior of Nonhuman Primates, Academic Press, New York, pp. 83–153.Google Scholar
  82. Milgram, S.: 1974. Obedience to Authority, Harper and Row, New York.Google Scholar
  83. Mitchell, R. W.: 1986, 'A Framework for Discussing Deception', in R. W. Mitchell and N. S. Thompson (eds), Deception: Perspectives on Human and Nonhuman Deceit, SUNY Press, New York, pp. 3–40.Google Scholar
  84. Nishida, T.: 1983, 'Alpha Status and Agonistic Alliance in Wild Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes Schweinfurthü)', Primates 24, 318–336.Google Scholar
  85. Nucci, L. P., E. Turiel and G. Encarnacion-Gawrych: 1983, 'Children's Social Interactions and Social Concepts', Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 14, 469–87.Google Scholar
  86. Oaksford, M. and N. Chater: 1994, 'A Rational Analysis of the Selection Task As Optimal Data Selection', Psychological Review 101, 608–31Google Scholar
  87. Perner, J.: 1991, Understanding the Representational Mind, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  88. Perner, J., T. Ruthman and S. R. Leekam: 1994, 'Theory of Mind is Contagious: You Catch It from Your Sibs', Child Development 65, 1228–38.Google Scholar
  89. Perner, J. and H. Wimmer: 1985, 'John Thinks That Mary Thinks That...: Attribution of Second-Order Beliefs by 5-10-Year-Old Children', Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 38, 437–71.Google Scholar
  90. Pope Edwards, C.: 1982, 'Moral Development in Comparative Cultural Perspective', in D. A. Wagner and H. W. Stevenson (eds), Cultural Perspectives on Child Development, W. H. Freeman, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  91. Politzer, G. and A. Nguyen-Xuan: 1992, 'Reasoning about Conditional Promises and Warnings: Darwinian Algorithms, Mental Models, Relevance Judgments, or Pragmatic Schemes?', Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 44A, 401–421.Google Scholar
  92. Povinelli, D. J.: 1994, 'What Chimpanzees (Might) Know about the Mind', in R. W. Wrangham, W. C. McGrew, F. B. M. de Waal and P. G. Heitne (eds), Chimpanzee Cultures, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 285–300.Google Scholar
  93. Prud'homme, J. and B. Chapais: 1993, 'Aggressive Interventions and Matrilineal Dominance Relations in Semifree-Ranging Barbary Macaques', Primates 34, 271–83.Google Scholar
  94. Putnam, H.: 1974, 'The 'Corroboration' of Theories', in P. A. Schilpp (ed.), The Philosophy of Karl Popper, Vol. 1, Open Court, LaSalle, IL, pp. 221–40.Google Scholar
  95. Rabbie, J. M.: 1992, 'The Effect of Intragroup Cooperation and Intergroup Competition on In-Group and Out-Group Hostility', in F. de Waal and A. H. Harcourt (eds), Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 175–208.Google Scholar
  96. Riggs, K. J., D. M. Peterson, E. J. Robinson, and P. Mitchell: 1998, 'Are Errors in False Belief Tasks Symptomatic of a Broader Difficulty with Counterfactuality?', Cognitive Development 13, 73–90.Google Scholar
  97. Riss, D. C. and J. Goodall: 1977, 'The Recent Rise to the Alpha-Rank in a Population of Free-Living Chimpanzees', Folia Primatologica 27, 134–51.Google Scholar
  98. Robinson, J. G.: 1982, 'Intrasexual Competition and Mate Choice in Primates', American Journal of Primatology, 1 Supplement, 131–44.Google Scholar
  99. Rubin, K. and R. J. Caplan: 1992, Peer Relationships in Childhood, Erlbaum, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.Google Scholar
  100. Ruffman, T., J. Perner, M. Naito, L. Parkin and W. C. Clements: 1998, 'Older (But Not Younger) Siblings Facilitate False Belief Understanding', Developmental Psychology 34, 161–74.Google Scholar
  101. Scribner, S.: 1975, 'Recall of Classical Syllogisms: A Cross-Cultural Investigation of Errors on Logical Problems', in R. J. Falmagne (ed.), Reasoning: Representation and Process in Children and Adults, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
  102. Seyfarth, R. M.: 1976, 'Social Relationships among Adult Female Monkeys', Animal Behavior 24, 917–38.Google Scholar
  103. Seyfarth, R. M. and D. L. Cheney: 1984, 'Grooming, Alliances, and Reciprocal Altriusm in Vervet Monkeys', Nature 308, 541–3.Google Scholar
  104. Silk, J. B.: 1987, 'Social Behavior in Evolutionary Perspective', in B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham and T. T. Struhsaker (eds), Primate Societies, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 318–29.Google Scholar
  105. Smith, P. K.: 1988, 'The Cognitive Demands of Children's Social Interactions With Peers', in R. W. Byrne and A. Whiten (eds), Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys Apes and Humans, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 94–110.Google Scholar
  106. Smuts, B.: 1985, Sex and Friendship in Baboons, Aldine Press, Hawthorne.Google Scholar
  107. Snarey, J. R.: 1985, 'Cross-Cultural Universality of Social-Moral Development: A Critical Review of Kohlbergian Research', Psychological Bulletin 97, 202–32.Google Scholar
  108. Stanovich, K. E. and R. F. West, in press, 'Cognitive Ability and Variation in Selection Task Performance', Thinking and Reasoning 4, 193-230.Google Scholar
  109. Strayer, F. F. and M. Trudel: 1984, 'Developmental Changes in the Nature and Function of Social Dominance Among Young Children', Ethology and Sociobiology 5, 279–95.Google Scholar
  110. Strichartz, A. F. and R. V. Burton: 1990, 'Lies and Truth: A Study in the Development of the Concept', Child Development 61, 211–20.Google Scholar
  111. Tutin, C. E. G.: 1979, 'Mating Patterns and Reproductive Strategies in a Community of Wild Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes Schweinfurtü)', Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 6, 29–38.Google Scholar
  112. Uehara, S., M. Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, K. Hosaka and M. Hamai: 1994, 'The Fate of Defeated Alpha Male Chimpanzees in Relation To Their Social Networks', Primates 35, 49–55.Google Scholar
  113. Vandell, L. and K. S. Wilson: (1987), 'Infant's InteractionsWith Mother, Sibling, and Peer: Contrasts and Relations Between Interaction Systems', Child Development 58, 176–86.Google Scholar
  114. de Waal, F. B.M.: 1982, Chimpanzee Politics, Johns-Hopkins University Press, Baltimore.Google Scholar
  115. de Waal, F. B. M.: 1989, 'Food Sharing and Reciprocal Obligations among Chimpanzees', Journal of Human Evolution 18, 433–59.Google Scholar
  116. de Waal, F. B. M.: 1992, 'Coalitions As Part of Reciprocal Relations in the Arnhem Chimpanzee Colony', in A. H. Harcourt and F. De Waal (eds), Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals, Oxford University, Oxford, pp. 233–58.Google Scholar
  117. de Waal, F. B. M.: 1996, Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  118. Waddington, C. H.: 1975, The Evolution of an Evolutionist, Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.Google Scholar
  119. Walters, J. R. and R. M. Seyfarth: 1987, 'Conflict and Cooperation', in B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R.M. Seyfarth, R.W. Wrangham, and T. T. Struhsaker (eds), Primate Societies, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 306–17.Google Scholar
  120. Watts, C. R. and A. W. Stokes: 1971, 'The Social Order of Turkeys', Scientific American 224, 112–8Google Scholar
  121. Weg, E. and V. Smith: 1993, 'On the Failure To Induce Meager Offers in Ultimatum Games', Journal of Economic Psychology 14, 17–32Google Scholar
  122. Wellman, H. M.: 1990, The Child's Theory of Mind, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  123. Whiten, A. and R. W. Byrne: 1988, 'The Manipulation of Attention in Primate Tactical Deception', in R. W. Byrne and A. Whiten (eds), Machiavellian Intelligence, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 211–24.Google Scholar
  124. Wimmer, H., S. Gruber and J. Perner: 1984, 'Young Children's Conception of Lying: Lexical Realism - Moral Subjectivism', Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 37, 1–30.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Denise Dellarosa Cummins
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyUniversity of California-DavisDavis

Personalised recommendations