Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 28, Issue 2, pp 193–204

“The Frog Ate the Bug and Made his Mouth Sad”: Narrative Competence in Children with Autism

  • Lisa Capps
  • Molly Losh
  • Christopher Thurber
Article

Abstract

This study compares the narrative abilities of 13 children with autism, 13 children with developmental delays, and 13 typically developing children matched on language ability. Although groups did not differ in their use of causal language or internal state terms, children with autism and children with developmental delays were less likely than typical children to identify the causes of characters' internal states. Rather, they tended simply to label emotions and explain actions. Children with autism and children with developmental delays also relied on a more restricted range of evaluative devices, which both convey point of view and maintain listener involvement. In addition, the narrative abilities of children with autism were linked to performance on measures of theory of mind and an index of conversational competence, whereas this was not the case among children with developmental delays. Findings are discussed in relation to the social, cognitive, and emotional underpinnings and consequences of narrative activity.

Autism narrative theory of mind conversational competence 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (3rd. ed., revised). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Bamberg, M. (1991). Narrative activity as perspective taking: The role of emotionals, negations, and voice in the construction of the story realm. Special Issue: Narrative. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 5, 275-290.Google Scholar
  3. Bamberg, M., & Marchman, V. (1991). Binding and unfolding: Towards the linguistic construction of narrative discourse. Discourse Processes, 14, 277-305.Google Scholar
  4. Bamberg, M., & Reilly, J. (1996). Emotion, narrative and affect: How children discover the relationship between what to say and how to say it. In D. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis, & J. Guo (Eds.), Interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 329-341). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 30, 285-297.Google Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S. Leslie, A., & Frith U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37-46.Google Scholar
  7. Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  8. Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child's talk. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  9. Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Bruner, J. S., & Feldman, C. (1993). Theories of mind and the problem of autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and developmental delay. Autism, 2, 325-344.Google Scholar
  13. Fein, D., Pennington, B., Markowitz, P., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse, L. (1986). Toward a neuropsychological model of infantile autism: Are the social deficits primary? Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25, 198-212.Google Scholar
  14. Feldman, C. F. (1989). Monologue as problem-solving narrative. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  15. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  16. Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  17. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 61-85.Google Scholar
  18. Krug, D. A., Arick, J. R., & Almond, P. J. (1978). Autism screening instrument for educational planning. Portland, OR: ASIEP Educational Co.Google Scholar
  19. Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1968). Narrative analysis. In W. Labov et al. (Eds.), A study of the non-standard English of Negro and Puerto Rican speakers in New York City (pp. 286-338). New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson, S., Holdrafter, M., & McLennan, J. (1989). Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363-387.Google Scholar
  21. Loveland, K., McEvoy, R., Tunali, B., & Kelley, M. L. (1990). Narrative story-telling in autism and Down syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 9-23.Google Scholar
  22. Loveland, K., & Tunali, B. (1993). Narrative Language in autism and the theory of mind hypothesis: A wider perspective. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. MacWhinney, B. (1990). The CHILDES Project: Computational tools for analyzing talk. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Mellon University.Google Scholar
  24. Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
  25. Mayer, M. (1973). Frog on his own. New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
  26. Miller, J. F. (1987). Language and communication characteristics of children with Down syndrome. In S. M. Pueschel, C. Tingley, J. E. Rynders, A. C. Crocker, & D. M. Crutcher (Eds.), New perspectives on Down syndrome (pp. 233-262). Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
  27. Miranda, A., Camp, L., Hemphill, L., & Wolf, D. (1992, May). Developmental changes in children's use of tense in narrative. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
  28. Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (1996). Narrating the self. Annual Review of Anthropology Palo Alto, 25, 19-43.Google Scholar
  29. Ochs, E. Taylor, C., Rudolph, D., & Smith, R. (1992). Storytelling as a theory-building activity. Discourse Processes, 15, 17-72.Google Scholar
  30. Polkinghorn, D. E. (1988). Narrative Knowing and the Human Sciences. Albany: State University of NY.Google Scholar
  31. Propp, V. (1968). The morphology of the folktale. (T. Scott, Trans.). Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  32. Reilly, J. S., Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1990) Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 367-391.Google Scholar
  33. Ricoeur, P. (1988). Time and narrative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  34. Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1986). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale. New York: Irvington Publishers.Google Scholar
  35. Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1987). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals-revised. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  36. Sigman, M., & Capps, L. (1997). Children with autism: A developmental perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Sigman, R., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64 (1 Serial N. 256).Google Scholar
  38. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children's talk about psychological states: Deficits in early acquisition of theory of mind. Child Development, 63, 161-172.Google Scholar
  39. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). What language reveals about the understanding of mind in children with autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. J. Cohen (Eds), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1995). 'Once upon a ribbit': Stories narrated by autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 45-59.Google Scholar
  41. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1994). Predicting and explaining behavior: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1059-1075.Google Scholar
  42. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison on narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 241-256.Google Scholar
  43. Thorndike, R., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale. Chicago: Riverside.Google Scholar
  44. Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Metaanalyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 283-307.Google Scholar
  45. Wang, P., & Bellugi, U. (1994). Evidence from two genetic syndromes for a dissociation between verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 317-322.Google Scholar
  46. White, H. (1981). The value of narrativity in the representation of reality. In W. J. T. Mitchell (Ed.), On narrative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  47. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lisa Capps
    • 1
  • Molly Losh
    • 1
    • 2
  • Christopher Thurber
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CaliforniaBerkeleyU.S.A.
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CaliforniaBerkeley
  3. 3.Phillips Exeter AcademyExeterU.S.A.

Personalised recommendations