Advertisement

Educational Studies in Mathematics

, Volume 39, Issue 1–3, pp 45–65 | Cite as

Embodied cognition as grounding for situatedness and context in mathematics education

  • Rafael E. Núñez
  • Laurie D. Edwards
  • João Filipe Matos
Article

Abstract

In this paper we analyze, from the perspective of 'Embodied Cognition', why learning and cognition are situated and context-dependent. We argue that the nature of situated learning and cognition cannot be fully understood by focusing only on social, cultural and contextual factors. These factors are themselves further situated and made comprehensible by the shared biology and fundamental bodily experiences of human beings. Thus cognition itself is embodied, and the bodily-grounded nature of cognition provides a foundation for social situatedness, entails a reconceptualization of cognition and mathematics itself, and has important consequences for mathematics education. After framing some theoretical notions of embodied cognition in the perspective of modern cognitive science, we analyze a case study – continuity of functions. We use conceptual metaphor theory to show how embodied cognition, while providing grounding for situatedness, also gives fruitful results in analyzing the cognitive difficulties underlying the understanding of continuity.

Keywords

Mathematics Education Cognitive Science Conceptual Mapping Conceptual System Bodily Experience 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Brooks, R. and Stein, L. A.: 1993, Building Brains for Bodies, A.I. Memo No. 1439. Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Artificial Intelligence Laboratory.Google Scholar
  2. Cobb, P.: 1994, 'Where is the mind? Constructivist and sociocultural perspectives on mathematical development', Educational Researcher 23(7), 13–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cobb, P., Yackel, E. and Wood, T.: 1992, 'A constructivist alternative to the representational view of mind', Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 23(1), 2–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Collins, A., Brown, J.S. and Newman, S.: 1989, 'Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching students the craft of reading, writing, and mathematics', in L.B. Resnick (ed.), Knowing, Learning, and Instruction: Essays in Honor of Robert Glaser Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 453–494.Google Scholar
  5. Confrey, J.: 1990, 'A review of the research on student conceptions in mathematics, science, and programming', in C. Cazden (ed.), Review of Research in Education, 16, American Educational Research Association, Washington, DC, pp. 3–56.Google Scholar
  6. Confrey, J.: 1995, 'A theory of intellectual development', For the Learning of Mathematics 15(1), 38–48.Google Scholar
  7. Csordas, T.J.: 1994, Embodiment and Experience: The Existential Ground of Culture and Self, University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  8. DiSessa, A, 1983, 'Phenomenology and the evolution of intuition', in D. Gentner and A. Stevens (eds.), Mental Models, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 15–33.Google Scholar
  9. Edelman, G.: 1992, Bright Air, Brilliant Fire, Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  10. Edwards, L. and Núñez, R.: 1995, 'Cognitive science and mathematics education: A non-objectivist view', Proceedings of the 19th conference of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education, PME. Vol. 2, pp. 240–247.Google Scholar
  11. Eysenk, M. and Keane, M.: 1992, Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Manual, Erlbaum, London.Google Scholar
  12. Feldman, J., Lakoff, G., Bailey, D., Narayanan, S., Regier, T. and Stolcke, A.: 1996, 'L0 — The first five years of an automated language acquisition project', Artificial Intelligence Review 10(1).Google Scholar
  13. Gardner, H.: 1985, The Mind's New Science, Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  14. Greeno, J.: 1997, 'On claims that answer the wrong questions', Educational Researcher, 26, 5–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Johnson, M.: 1987, The Body in the Mind, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  16. Kitcher, P.: 1997, April, Personal communication, University of California at San Diego.Google Scholar
  17. Lakoff, G.: 1987, Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  18. Lakoff, G.: 1993, 'The contemporary theory of metaphor', in A. Ortony (ed.) Metaphor and Thought, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 202–251.Google Scholar
  19. Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M.: 1980, Metaphors we Live by, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  20. Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M.: 1998, Philosophy in the Flesh, Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  21. Lakoff, G. and Núñez, R., 1997, 'The metaphorical structure of mathematics: Sketching out cognitive foundations for a mind-based mathematics', in L. English (ed.), Mathematical Reasoning: Analogies, Metaphors, and Images, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 21–89.Google Scholar
  22. Lakoff, G. and Núñez, R.: forthcoming, Where Mathematics Comes From: How the Embodied Mind Creates Mathematics.Google Scholar
  23. Lave, J.: 1988, Cognition in Practice: Mind, Mathematics and Culture in Everyday Life, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  24. Lave, J. and Wenger, E.: 1991, Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  25. Lock, M.: 1993, 'Cultivating the body: Anthropology and epistemologies of bodily practice and knowledge', Annual Review of Anthropology 22, 133–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Maturana, H. and Varela, F.: 1987, The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding, New Science Library, Boston.Google Scholar
  27. Merleau-Ponty, M.: 1945, La Phénoménologie de la Perception, Gallimard, Paris.Google Scholar
  28. Nunes, T. and Bryant, P.: 1996, Children Doing Mathematics, Blackwells, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  29. Núñez, R.: 1993, En deçà du transfini: Aspects psychocognitifs sous-jacents au concept d'infini en mathématiques, University Press, Fribourg, Switzerland.Google Scholar
  30. Núñez, R.: 1995, 'What brain for God's-eye? Biological naturalism, ontological objectivism, and searle', Journal of Consciousness Studies 2(2), 149–166.Google Scholar
  31. Núñez, R.: 1997, 'Eating soup with chopsticks: Dogmas, difficulties, and alternatives in the study of conscious experience', Journal of Consciousness Studies 4(2), 143–166.Google Scholar
  32. Núñez, R. and Lakoff, G.: 1998, 'What did weierstrass really define? The cognitive structure of natural and εδ continuity', Mathematical Cognition 4(2), 85–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pierpont, J.: 1899, 'On the arithmetization of mathematics', Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society, 394–406.Google Scholar
  34. Regier, T.: 1996, The Human Semantic Potential, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  35. Robert, A.: 1982, 'L'acquisition de la notion de convergence de suites numériques dans l'enseignement supérieur', Recherches en didactique des mathématiques, 3, 307–341.Google Scholar
  36. Rogoff, B.: 1990, Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context, Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  37. Rogoff, B. and Lave, J. (eds.): 1984, Everyday Cognition: Its Development in Social Context, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  38. Rosch, E.: 1973, 'Natural categories', Cognitive Psychology 4, 328–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Rosch, E., 1994, 'Categorization', in V.S. Ramachandran (ed.), The Encyclopedia of Human Behavior, Academic Press, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
  40. Sanford, A.: 1985, Cognition and Cognitive Psychology, Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  41. Simmons, G.F.: 1985, Calculus with Analytic Geometry, McGraw-Hill, New York.Google Scholar
  42. Smith, J.P, diSessa, A. and Roschelle, J.: 1993/94, 'Misconceptions reconceived: A constructivist analysis of knowledge in transition', Journal of the Learning Sciences 3(2), 115–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Tall, D.O. and Vinner, S.: 1981, 'Concept image and concept definition in mathematics with particular reference to limits and continuity', Educational Studies in Mathematics 12, 151–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Talmy, L.: 1988, 'Force dynamics in language and cognition', Cognitive Science, 12(1), 49–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Thelen, E. and Smith, L.: 1994, A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  46. Varela, F., Thompson, E. and Rosch, E.: 1991, The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  47. Von Glasersfeld, E.: 1990, 'An exposition of constructivism: Why some like it radical', in R. Davis, C. Maher and N. Noddings (eds.), Constructivist Views on the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics, National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Reston, VA.Google Scholar
  48. Walkerdine, V.: 1982, 'From context to text: A psychosemiotic approach to abstract thought', in M. Beveridge (ed.), Children Thinking through Language, Edward Arnold, London.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1999

Authors and Affiliations

  • Rafael E. Núñez
    • 1
  • Laurie D. Edwards
    • 2
  • João Filipe Matos
    • 3
  1. 1.Institute of Cognitive StudiesBerkeleyU.S.A.
  2. 2.School of EducationSt. Mary's College of CaliforniaMoragaU.S.A
  3. 3.Department of EducationUniversity of LisbonPortugal

Personalised recommendations