Spiral and Helical Models for Psychology: Leaving Linearity Behind

Abstract

We suggest that theoretical models in the social sciences would benefit from uses of nature’s images that map the complexity of the phenomena to be investigated. Such abstractions would better maintain the open-systemic character of the psychological and social phenomena in all their complexities. Particularly central in such complexities are dynamic catalytic processes that are operating in wholistic fields of psychological systems such as self, identity, and values Theoretical models taken from the mechanical realms of computational processes fail to capture these constantly changing and often nebulous fields. In particular, we analyze the promises of two abstract forms based on nature—spiral and helix—as providing temporal structure for understanding basic higher psychological functions, using the dialogical self theory as an example. The focus of that theory on dialogicality between I-positions in their transition through ruptures of the whole system is best fitted with nonlinear field-dependent models. For all systems of complex dynamic wholes kind, theoretical field models emulating various biocynotic systems (“meadow,” “mychorrea,” etc.) would constitute a new direction in theoretical advancement in the human sciences.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12

Notes

  1. 1.

    Interestingly, the branch uniting the spiral outgrowths that was characteristic of Ancient Greek and Roman ornaments disappeared in Byzantine versions of ornamenting practices. The psychological significance of such ever-present patterns indicates the curvilinear nature of human internal affective processes (Valsiner, 2018).

  2. 2.

    Blühende Natur” describable as plants, animals, waters, and rocks. Which means all-natural complex that is part of the earth's surface.

  3. 3.

    Unbelebte Natur” definable as everything, from physical and chemical forces to biological organism, that exists or develops in organic and inorganic phenomena without human intervention.

References

  1. Acerbi, F. (2013). Aristotle and Euclid’s postulates. The Classical Quarterly, 63(2), 680–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Bang, J., & Winther-Lindqvist, D. (Eds.). (2016). Nothingness. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Beloussov, L. (1998). The dynamic architecture of a developing organism. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Cabell, K. R., and Valsiner, J. (Eds.) (2014). The catalyzing mind: Beyond models of causality. Vol, 11 of Advances of Theoretical Psychology. New York: Springer.

  5. Campbell, J. (2002). The flight of the wild gander. (p. 117, Illustrated, Reprint (Revised). Novato, CA: New World Library.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Campill, M. A. (2021). Towards a Wholistic Model of Identity: why Not a Meadow? Integrative psychological and Behavioral Science., 55(1), 112–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Campill, M. A. and von Fircks, E, (in preparation). The pluralistic-self. To be submitted to Journal of Constructivist Research.

  8. Cavallaro, D. (2015). The art of Studio Gainax: experimentation, style and innovation at the leading edge of anime. McFarland.

  9. Dazai, O. (1958). No longer human (Vol. 357). New Directions Publishing.

  10. Deutsch, M. (1954). Field theory in social psychology. Handbook of social psychology, 1, 181–222.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Dunlap, R. A. (1997). The golden ratio and Fibonacci numbers. World Scientific.

  12. Engeström, Y. (2006). Development, movement and agency: Breaking away into mycorrhizae activities. Building activity theory in practice: Toward the next generation, 1, 1–43.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Fleckenstein, J. O. (1975). 8.2. Kepler and Neoplatonism. Vistas in Astronomy, 18, 427–438.

  14. Fujisawa, C. (1959). Zen and Shinto (p. 92). The Story of Japanese Philosophy. New York: Philosophical Library.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Harcourt, H. M. (2019). The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Fifth Edition: Fiftieth Anniversary Printing. London: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Harvey, D. C. (2003). ‘National’ identities and the politics of ancient heritage: continuity and change at ancient monuments in Britain and Ireland, c. 1675–1850. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, 28(4), 473–487.

  17. Heinich, N. (1997). The glory of Van Gogh: An anthropology of admiration. Princeton University Press.

  18. Hermans, H. (2001). The dialogical self: Toward a theory of personal and cultural -. Culture & Psychology, 7(3), 243–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ingold, T. (2016). Lines. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Lamiell, J. (1998). Nomothetic’ and ‘idiographic’: Contrasting Windelband’s understanding with contemporary usage. Theory & Psychology, 8, 23–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Lewin, K. (1930–1931). Der Übergang von der aristotelischen zur galileischen Denkweise in Biologie und Psychologie. Erkenntnis, 1, . 421–466.

  22. Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. Durham: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Michell, J. (1999). Measurement in psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Mittelstraß, J. (Ed.). (2016). Enzyklopädie Philosophie und Wissenschaftstheorie: Bd. 5: Log–N. Springer-Verlag.

  25. Nir, D. (2012). Voicing inner conflict: From a dialogical to a negotiational self. In H. Hermans & T. Gieser (Eds.), Handbook of dialogical self theory (pp. 284–300). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Popov, A., & Iacob, A. (2014). Lobachevsky geometry and modern nonlinear problems. Switzerland: Birkhäuser.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Puche Navarro, R. (Ed.). (2009). Es la mente no lineal? Cali: Programa editorial Universidad del Valle.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Rayner, A. (2017). The origin of life patterns in the natural inclusion of space in flux. Cham, CH: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Taioli, S., Gabbrielli, R., Simonucci, S., Pugno, N. M., & Iorio, A. (2016). Lobachevsky crystallography made real through carbon pseudospheres. Journal of Physics: Condensed Matter, 28(13), 13LT01

  30. Thompson, D’A. . (1917). On growth and form. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Toomela, A., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2010). Methodological thinking in psychology: 60 years gone astray? Charlotte, N.C.: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Tsuda, I. (2001). Toward an interpretation of dynamic neutral activity in terms of chaotic dynamical systems. Behvioral and Brain Science, 24, 793–847.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Valsiner, J. (2005). Attractors, repulsors, and directors: making Dynamic Systems Theory developmental. Annual Report 2003–2004 of Research and Clinical Center for Child Development, Graduate School of Education, Hokkaido University. Sapporo, No., 27, 13–35.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Valsiner, J. (2018). Ornamented lives. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Valsiner, J. (2019). From causality to catalysis in the social sciences. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Social philosophy of science for the social sciences (pp. 125–146). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Valsiner, J. (2020). Sensuality in human living: The cultural psychology of affect. Cham, CH: Springer Nature (Springer Briefs- Theoretical Advances in Psychology). ISBN 978–3–030–41742–0

  37. Vogel, H. (1979). A better way to construct the sunflower head. Mathematical biosciences, 44(3–4), 179–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Wilson, C. (2000). From Kepler to Newton: Telling the tale. The Foundations of Newtonian Scholarship, 223–242.

  39. Zittoun, T. (2006). Transitions: Symbolic resources in development. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We want to acknowledge the second author’s laptop that made the development of the dialectical ideas in this article so solid and did not let us down.

Funding

The author received no financial support for the research and publication of this article, it stands on its own as a virginal creative act that is often the case in contemporary academia.

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marc Antoine Campill.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

This study does not contain any studies with human participants or animals by the authors.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Campill, M.A., Valsiner, J. Spiral and Helical Models for Psychology: Leaving Linearity Behind. Hu Arenas (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-021-00194-2

Download citation

Keywords

  • Spiral
  • Helix
  • Nonlinearity
  • Hyper generalized sign-field
  • Identity
  • I-positioning
  • Organic metaphors
  • Polysemic multivoice
  • Self-identification meadow