Advertisement

Activitas Nervosa Superior

, Volume 60, Issue 1, pp 8–17 | Cite as

The High-Order and Conscious Emotion: Assessing the Foundations, Contributions, and Implications of LeDoux’s Model of Conscious and Cognitive Emotion

  • Luiz Augusto Rosa
Ideas and Perpectives

Abstract

In the literature on emotion, it is common to associate this phenomenon with unconscious and/or subcortical mechanisms directly linked to survival circuits, which is recurringly called “traditional view of emotion.” However, LeDoux and Brown have recently proposed that emotion is a high-order and cortical process carried out by what he calls general networks of cognition (GNC), which consists mainly of prefrontal mechanisms. Here, I assess the new ideas brought by this model to the field. I assess on what this model is based, its explanatory power, and what it means for the interpretation and discussion of empirical data. I also discuss how LeDoux and Brown's model of conscious emotion explains and interprets some recurring topics in the literature on emotion, such as nonhuman emotional experience, prefrontal functioning, freezing behavior, emotion regulation, and pleasure mechanisms. Above all, my goal is to provide theoretical tools for empirical and theoretical discussions on this recent issue.

Keywords

Emotion Cognition Consciousness High-order representation Theoretical model HOTEC 

Notes

Funding Information

This paper was supported by São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP).

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aggleton, J. P. (1993). The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional states. Trends in Neurosciences, 16, 328–333.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(93)90110-8.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Agrillo, C., Miletto Petrazzini, M. E., Piffer, L., Dadda, M., & Bisazza, A. (2012). A new training procedure for studying discrimination learning in fish. Behavioural Brain Research, 230, 343–348.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.02.025.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson, D. J., & Adolphs, R. (2014). A framework for studying emotions across phylogeny. Cell, 157, 187–200.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.003.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2002). Is the human amygdala critical for the subjective experience of emotion? Evidence of intact dispositional affect in patients with amygdala lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 709–720.  https://doi.org/10.1162/08989290260138618.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Anderson, V. A., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R., & Catroppa, C. (2001). Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 385–406.  https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN2001_5.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Aru, J., Bachmann, T., Singer, W., & Melloni, L. (2012). Distilling the neural correlates of consciousness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 737–746.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.003.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Balleine, B. W., Delgado, M. R., & Hikosaka, O. (2007). The role of the dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 8161–8165.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1554-07.2007.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Banks, S. J., Eddy, K. T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P. J., & Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala—frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 303–312.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm029.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Barrett, L. F. (2006a). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 28–58.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Barrett, L. F. (2006b). Solving the emotion paradox: categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 20–46.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Barrett, L. F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1–23.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw154.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. A., Bliss-Moreau, E., Duncan, S., Gendron, M., Mize, J., & Brennan, L. (2007). Of mice and men: natural kinds of emotions in the mammalian brain? A response to Panksepp and izard. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 297–312.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00046.x.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Baxter, M. G., & Murray, E. A. (2002). The amygdala and reward. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3, 563–573.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn875.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295–307.  https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/10.3.295.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2008). Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology, 199, 457–480.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-008-1099-6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2015). Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron, 86, 646–664.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1969). Crouching as an index of fear. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 67, 370–375.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026779.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Bornemann, B., Winkielman, P., & van der Meer, E. (2012). Can you feel what you do not see? Using internal feedback to detect briefly presented emotional stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 85, 116–124.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.04.007.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Brickner, R. M. (1952). Brain of patient A. After bilateral frontal lobectomy; status of frontal-lobe problem. A.M.A. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 68, 293–313.  https://doi.org/10.1001/archneurpsyc.1952.02320210003001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Brown, R. (2015). The HOROR theory of phenomenal consciousness. Philosophical Studies, 172, 1783–1794.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-014-0388-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 215–222.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
  24. Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. San Diego: Harcourt.Google Scholar
  25. Damasio, A. R. (2010). Self comes to mind: constructing the conscious brain. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
  26. Damasio, A. R., & Carvalho, G. B. (2013). The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 143–152.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3403.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: Fontana Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Dehaene, S. (2014). Consciousness and the brain: deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. New York: Viking Adult.Google Scholar
  29. Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 70, 200–227.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79, 1–37.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.-P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211.Google Scholar
  32. Del Cul, A., Dehaene, S., Reyes, P., Bravo, E., & Slachevsky, A. (2009). Causal role of prefrontal cortex in the threshold for access to consciousness. Brain, 132, 2531–2540.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp111.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & van Baaren, R. B. (2006). On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science, 311, 1005–1007.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1121629.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Doya, K., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1995). A novel reinforcement model of birdsong vocalization learning. In G. Tesauro, D. S. Touretzky, & T. K. Leen (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems (Vol. 7, pp. 101–108). Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  35. Duncan, S., & Barrett, L. F. (2007). Affect is a form of cognition: a neurobiological analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1184–1211.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701437931.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotions. In J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1971 (pp. 207–283). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  37. Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and nature of emotion. In K. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 319–343). Hillsdale: Earlbaum.Google Scholar
  38. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ekman, E., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 1208–1210.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6612338.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Etkin, A., Egner, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R., & Hirsch, J. (2006). Resolving emotional conflict: a role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron, 51, 871–882.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.029.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Etkin, A., Egner, T., & Kalisch, R. (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 85–93.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Fabbro, F., Aglioti, S. M., Bergamasco, M., Clarici, A., & Panksepp, J. (2015). Evolutionary aspects of self- and world consciousness in vertebrates. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 157.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00157.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Feinstein, J. S., Buzza, C., Hurlemann, R., Follmer, R. L., Dahdaleh, N. S., Coryell, W. H., et al. (2013). Fear and panic in humans with bilateral amygdala damage. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 270–272.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3323.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Frässle, S., Sommer, J., Jansen, A., Naber, M., & Einhäuser, W. (2014). Binocular rivalry: frontal activity relates to introspection and action but not to perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 1738–1747.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4403-13.2014.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Frith, C., Perry, R., & Lumer, E. (1999). The neural correlates of conscious experience: an experimental framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 105–114.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01281-4.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Gale, J. T., Shields, D. C., Ishizawa, Y., & Eskandar, E. N. (2014). Reward and reinforcement activity in the nucleus accumbens during learning. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 114.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00114.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Gibson, W. T., Gonzalez, C. R., Fernandez, C., Ramasamy, L., Tabachnik, T., Du, R. R., et al. (2015). Behavioral responses to a repetitive visual threat stimulus express a persistent state of defensive arousal in Drosophila. Current Biology, 25, 1401–1415.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.058.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269–288.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939008410799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Graybiel, A. M. (2005). The basal ganglia: learning new tricks and loving it. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 638–644.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.006.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Gross, J. J. (2013). Emotion regulation: taking stock and moving forward. Emotion, 13, 359–365.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032135.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Gross, J. J., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regulation: one or two depends on your point of view. Emotion Review, 3, 8–16.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910380974.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Güntürkün, O., & Bugnyar, T. (2016). Cognition without cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 291–303.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.02.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Hall, C. S. (1934). Emotional behavior in the rat. I. Defecation and urination as measures of individual differences in emotionality. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 18, 385–403.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0071444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Hogeveen, J., Salvi, C., & Grafman, J. (2016). ‘Emotional intelligence’: lessons from lesions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 39, 694–705.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2016.08.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion-cognition relations. Psychology Review, 99, 561–565.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Johnson, S. B., Blum, R. W., & Giedd, J. N. (2009). Adolescent maturity and the brain: the promise and pitfalls of neuroscience research in adolescent health policy. The Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 216–221.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.05.016.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2007). Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 16–22.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.012.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2016). Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 17, 307–321.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: a review. Biological Psychology, 84, 394–421.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Lau, H. C. (2008). A higher order Bayesian decision theory of consciousness. Progress in Brain Research, 168, 35–48.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68004-2.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Lazarus, R. S. (1981). A cognitivist’s reply to Zajonc on emotion and cognition. The American Psychologist, 36, 222–223.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.36.2.222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. The American Psychologist, 39, 124–129.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.2.124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. The American Psychologist, 46, 352–367.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.4.352.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. LeDoux, J. E. (1994). Emotion, memory and the brain. Scientific American, 270, 50–57.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 209–235.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
  67. LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155–184.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. LeDoux, J. E. (2012). Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron, 73, 653–676.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. LeDoux, J. E. (2014). Coming to terms with fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 2871–2878.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1400335111.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. LeDoux, J. E. (2015). Anxious: unising the brain to understand and treat fear and anxiety. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
  71. LeDoux, J. E., & Brown, R. (2017). A higher-order theory of emotional consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114, E2016–E2025.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1619316114.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. LeDoux, J. E., & Pine, D. S. (2016). Using neuroscience to help understand fear and anxiety: a two-system framework. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 1083–1093.  https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030353.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. LeDoux, J. E., Iwata, J., Cicchetti, P., & Reis, D. J. (1988). Different projections of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral correlates of conditioned fear. The Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 2517–2529.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Levenson, R. W. (2014). The autonomic nervous system and emotion. Emotion Review, 6, 100–112.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913512003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain, 106, 623–642.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Loonen, A. J. M., & Ivanova, S. A. (2015). Circuits regulating pleasure and happiness: the evolution of reward-seeking and misery-fleeing behavioral mechanisms in vertebrates. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 394.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2015.00394.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Morawetz, C., Bode, S., Baudewig, J., & Heekeren, H. R. (2017). Effective amygdala-prefrontal connectivity predicts individual differences in successful emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 569–585.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw169.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Morris, J. S., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 1680–1685.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.96.4.1680.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Morris, J. S., DeGelder, B., Weiskrantz, L., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. Brain, 124, 1241–1252.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/124.6.1241.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Naragon-Gainey, K., McMahon, T. P., & Chacko, T. P. (2017). The structure of common emotion regulation strategies: a meta-analytic examination. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 384–427.  https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000093.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Oatley, K., & Johnson-Iard, P. P. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 29–50.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938708408362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Olkowicz, S., Kocourek, M., Lučan, R. K., Porteš, M., Fitch, W. T., Herculano-Houzel, S., & Němec, P. (2016). Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 7255–7260.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1517131113.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Panksepp, J. (1982). Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 407–467.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0001275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  85. Panksepp, J. (2000). Emotions as natural kinds within the mammalian brain. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2th ed., pp. 137–156). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  86. Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 30–80.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Panksepp, J. (2010a). Affective consciousness in animals: perspectives on dimensional and primary process emotion approaches. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 277, 2905–2907.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Panksepp, J. (2010b). Affective neuroscience of the emotional BrainMind: evolutionary perspectives and implications for understanding depression. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 12, 533–545.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  89. Panksepp, J. (2011). Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PLoS One, 6, e21236.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021236.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Panksepp, J., Lane, R. D., Solms, M., & Smith, R. (2016). Reconciling cognitive and affective neuroscience perspectives on the brain basis of emotional experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 76, 187–215.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.010.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Paul, E.S., Harding, E.J., & Mendl, M. (2005). Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 469–491. doi:  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002.
  92. Pessoa, L. (2010). Emotion and cognition and the amygdala: from “what is it?” to “what’s to be done?”. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3416–3429.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.038.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Phelps, E. A., & LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48, 175–187.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Philippi, C. L., Feinstein, J. S., Khalsa, S. S., Damasio, A., Tranel, D., Landini, G., Williford, K., Rudrauf, D., Hoshi, Y. (2012). Preserved Self-Awareness following Extensive Bilateral Brain Damage to the Insula, Anterior Cingulate, and Medial Prefrontal Cortices. PLoS ONE, 7 (8), e38413.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038413
  95. Rempel-Clower, N. L. (2007). Role of orbitofrontal cortex connections in emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1121, 72–86.  https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1401.026.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Rosenthal, D. M. (2005). Consciousness and mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  97. Safavi, S., Kapoor, V., Logothetis, N., & Panagiotaropoulos, T. I. (2014). Is the frontal lobe involved in conscious perception? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1063.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01063.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Sandberg, K., Frässle, S., & Pitts, M. (2016). Future directions for identifying the neural correlates of consciousness. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 17, 666.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Schultz, W. (2015). Neuronal reward and decision signals: from theories to data. Physiological Reviews, 95, 853–951.  https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00023.2014.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Seitz, A. R., Kim, D., & Watanabe, T. (2009). Rewards evoke learning of unconsciously processed visual stimuli in adult humans. Neuron, 61, 700–707.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.016.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Silvers, J. A., Insel, C., Powers, A., Franz, P., Helion, C., Martin, R. E., et al. (2016). vlPFC-vmPFC-amygdala interactions underlie age-related differences in cognitive regulation of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 27, 3502–3514.  https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Smith, K. S., Tindell, A. J., Aldridge, J. W., & Berridge, K. C. (2009). Ventral pallidum roles in reward and motivation. Behavioural Brain Research, 196, 155–167.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.09.038.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Tamietto, M., & de Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 11, 697–709.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2889.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Tononi, G., Boly, M., Massimini, M., & Koch, C. (2016). Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 17, 450–461.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.44.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Tsuchiya, N., Wilke, M., Frässle, S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2015). No-report paradigms: extracting the true neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 757–770.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Vandekerckhove, M., & Panksepp, J. (2011). A neurocognitive theory of higher mental emergence: from anoetic affective experiences to noetic knowledge and autonoetic awareness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 2017–2025.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.001.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Wassum, K. M., & Izquierdo, A. (2015). The basolateral amygdala in reward learning and addiction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 271–283.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.017.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Williams, L. M., Das, P., Liddell, B. J., Kemp, A. H., Rennie, C. J., & Gordon, E. (2006). Mode of functional connectivity in amygdala pathways dissociates level of awareness for signals of fear. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 9264–9271.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1016-06.2006.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., & Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychological Science, 24, 947–956.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612464242.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Xue, X., Zhou, X., & Li, S. (2015). Unconscious reward facilitates motion perceptual learning. Visual Cognition, 23, 161–178.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2014.981625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117–123.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.2.117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Philosophy and SciencesSão Paulo State University (UNESP)São PauloBrazil

Personalised recommendations