Advertisement

Journal of Cognitive Enhancement

, Volume 2, Issue 2, pp 193–199 | Cite as

Exercise and Creativity: Can One Bout of Yoga Improve Convergent and Divergent Thinking?

  • Kathleen F. Donnegan
  • Annalisa Setti
  • Andrew P. AllenEmail author
Original Article

Abstract

While creativity is a vastly debated topic, little research has been dedicated to determining whether exercise can boost cognitive factors associated with creativity, such as divergent thinking. Yoga, as a form of exercise, comprises physical activity and open-monitoring meditation, which may increase divergent thinking. We compared performance on a test of divergent thinking in healthy adults, the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA), and one test of convergent thinking and field independence, the Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT), before and after one session of ashtanga yoga, and one session of aerobic exercise. Divergent thinking was not affected by either intervention overall; however, fluency of novel ideas generated was reduced post-intervention in both groups. Practice effects were registered for the convergent thinking task, and those in the yoga group performed better at this task both at baseline and following yoga, although yoga did not lead to a greater change from baseline performance. The current findings do not suggest that one bout of yoga is associated with an immediate cognitive benefit. However, further research is required onto whether long-term yoga practice may enhance divergent thinking.

Keywords

Creativity Divergent thinking Exercise Yoga 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge Amy Jordan and Maeve McLoughlin for assistance with data collection, as well as the gyms that hosted the exercise groups.

References

  1. Allen, A. P. (2011). The zoo of the new. The Psychologist, 24, 862–863.Google Scholar
  2. Allen, A. P., & Thomas, K. E. (2011). A dual process account of creative thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23(2), 109–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baas, M., Nevicka, B., & Ten Velden, F. S. (2014). Specific mindfulness skills differentially predict creative performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(9), 1092–1106.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(4), 309–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 701–725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Blanchette, D. M., Ramocki, S. P., O'del, J. N., & Casey, M. S. (2005). Aerobic exercise and creative potential: immediate and residual effects. Creativity Research Journal, 17(2–3), 257–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Booth, M. (2000). Assessment of physical activity: an international perspective. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71(sup2), 114–120.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the creative achievement questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 17(1), 37–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chang, Y.-K., Labban, J. D., Gapin, J. I., & Etnier, J. L. (2012). The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Research, 1453, 87–101.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Science, 14(2), 125–130.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Colzato, L. S., & Hommel, B. (2017). Meditation. In L. S. Colzato (Ed.), Theory-driven approaches to cognitive enhancement (pp. 225–236). Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Colzato, L. S., Ozturk, A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Meditate to create: the impact of focused-attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 116.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Colzato, L. S., Szapora Ozturk, A., Pannekoek, J. N., & Hommel, B. (2013). The impact of physical exercise on convergent and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 824.Google Scholar
  14. Cropley, D. H., Cropley, A. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Runco, M. A. (Eds.). (2010). The dark side of creativity. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Davis, J. K., & Haueisen, W. C. (1976). Field independence and hypothesis testing. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43(3), 763–769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ding, X., Tang, Y. Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2014). Improving creativity performance by short-term meditation. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 10(1), 9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Duncker, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5), (Whole No. 270), 1–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dziedziewicz, D., Gajda, A., & Karwowski, M. (2014). Developing children's intercultural competence and creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 13, 32–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dziedziewicz, D., Oledzka, D., & Karwowski, M. (2013). Developing 4-to 6-year-old children’s figural creativity using a doodle-book program. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 9, 85–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ferris, L. T., Williams, J. S., & Shen, C.-L. (2007). The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 39(4), 728–734.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Goff, K., & Torrance, E. P. (2002). Abbreviated Torrance test for adults. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.Google Scholar
  22. Guilford, J. P. (1957). Creative abilities in the arts. Psychological Review, 64(2), 110–118.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 58–65.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Howard-Jones, P. A. (2002). A dual-state model of creative cognition for supporting strategies that foster creativity in the classroom. International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 12(3), 215–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kasof, J. (1997). Creativity and breadth of attention. Creativity Research Journal, 10(4), 303–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kelly, M. E., Loughrey, D., Lawlor, B. A., Robertson, I. H., Walsh, C., & Brennan, S. (2014). The impact of exercise on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Research Reviews, 16, 12–31.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Khasky, A. D., & Smith, J. C. (1999). Stress, relaxation states, and creativity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 88(2), 409–416.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Lebuda, I., Zabelina, D. L., & Karwowski, M. (2016). Mind full of ideas: a meta-analysis of the mindfulness–creativity link. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 22–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lippelt, D. P., Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2014). Focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation: effects on attention, conflict monitoring, and creativity—a review. Frontiers in Psychology, 5.Google Scholar
  30. Marrocu, E., & Paci, R. (2012). Education or creativity: what matters most for economic performance? Economic Geography, 88(4), 369–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. O’Brien, J., Ottoboni, G., Tessari, A., & Setti, A. (2017). One bout of open skill exercise improves cross-modal perception and immediate memory in healthy older adults who habitually exercise. PLoS One, 12(6), e0178739.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0178739.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Schneider, W., & Chein, J. M. (2003). Controlled & automatic processing: behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cognitive Science, 27(3), 525–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Sowden, P. T., Pringle, A., & Gabora, L. (2015). The shifting sands of creative thinking: connections to dual-process theory. Thinking & Reasoning, 21(1), 40–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Sridevi, K., Sitamma, M., & Krishna Rao, P. V. (1995). Perceptual organisation and yoga training. Journal of Indian Psychology, 13(2), 21–27.Google Scholar
  35. Steinberg, H., Sykes, E. A., Moss, T., Lowery, S., LeBoutillier, N., & Dewey, A. (1997). Exercise enhances creativity independently of mood. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 31(3), 240–245.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Tomporowski, P. D. (2003). Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychologica, 112(3), 297–324.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Torrance, P. E. (1966/1999). The Torrance tests of creative thinking: Norms and technical manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic testing Services.Google Scholar
  38. Williams, L. K., & McGuire, S. J. (2010). Economic creativity and innovation implementation: the entrepreneurial drivers of growth? Evidence from 63 countries. Small Business Economics, 34(4), 391–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. A. (1971). A manual for the Group Embedded Figures Test. Menlo Park, CA: Mind Garden, Inc..Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Applied PsychologyUniversity College CorkCorkIreland
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyMaynooth UniversityMaynoothIreland

Personalised recommendations