Advertisement

Journal of Cognitive Enhancement

, Volume 2, Issue 3, pp 240–258 | Cite as

Similar Task-Switching Performance of Real-Time Strategy and First-Person Shooter Players: Implications for Cognitive Training

  • Annika L. KlaffehnEmail author
  • Katharina A. Schwarz
  • Wilfried Kunde
  • Roland PfisterEmail author
Original Article

Abstract

Computer games have been proposed as effective tools for cognitive enhancement. Especially first-person shooter (FPS) games have been found to yield a range of positive effects, and these positive effects also apply to the domain of executive functioning. Only a particular area of executive functioning has been shown to resist training via FPS games, and this area is task-switching performance. Here, we tested whether games of a different genre, real-time strategy (RTS) games, offer a more promising approach to improve task-switching performance, because RTS games capitalize on precisely this behavior. A high-powered, quasi-experimental comparison of RTS and FPS players indicated reliable costs for task-switching across both player groups—with similar performance on multiple indicators, comprising switch costs, mixing costs, voluntary switch rates, and psychological refractory period effects. Performance of both groups further did not exceed the performance of a control group of Chess and Go players. These results corroborate previous findings on the robustness of cognitive costs of task-switching. At the same time, our results also suggest that the precise characteristics of different computer games might not be critical in determining potential training effects.

Keywords

Task-switching Executive functions Computer games Cognitive training Cognitive enhancement 

References

  1. Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance series. Attention and performance 15. Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  2. Andrews, G., & Murphy, K. (2006). Does video-game playing improve executive function? In M. A. Vanchevsky (Ed.), Frontiers in cognitive psychology (Ch. 8, pp. 145–161). New York: Nova Science.Google Scholar
  3. Arrington, C. M., & Logan, G. D. (2004). The cost of a voluntary task switch. Psychological Science, 15(9), 610–615.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00728.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Basak, C., Boot, W. R., Voss, M. W., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Can training in a real-time strategy videogame attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 765–777.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013494.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Becker, M. W., Alzahabi, R., & Hopwood, C. J. (2013). Media multitasking is associated with symptoms of depression and social anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 16(2), 132–135.  https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0291.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bediou, B., Adams, D. M., Mayer, R. E., Tipton, E., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2017). Meta-analysis of action video game impact on perceptual, attentional, and cognitive skills. Psychological Bulletin. Advance online publication.  https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000130.
  7. Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2008). The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta Psychologica, 129(3), 387–398.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Boot, W. R., Blakely, D. P., & Simons, D. J. (2011). Do action video games improve perception and cognition? Frontiers in Psychology, 2.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00226.
  9. Cain, M. S., Landau, A. N., & Shimamura, A. P. (2012). Action video game experience reduces the cost of switching tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(4), 641–647.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0284-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cohen, J. E., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2008). Training visual attention with video games: not all games are created equal. In H. F. O’Neil & R. S. Perez (Eds.), Computer games and team and individual learning (pp. 205–227). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  11. Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Science, 14(2), 125–130.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Colzato, L. S., van Leeuwen, P. J. A., van den Wildenberg, W., & Hommel, B. (2010). DOOM’d to switch: superior cognitive flexibility in players of first person shooter games. Frontiers in Psychology, 1.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00008.
  13. Colzato, L. S., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Hommel, B. (2014). Cognitive control and the COMT Val158Met polymorphism: genetic modulation of videogame training and transfer to task-switching efficiency. Psychological Research, 78(5), 670–678.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0514-8.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Dale, G., & Green, C. S. (2016). Video games and cognitive performance. In R. Kowert & T. Quandt (Eds.), The video game debate: Unravelling the physical, social, and psychological effects of digital games (pp. 131–152). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  15. Dale, G., & Green, C. S. (2017). Associations between avid action and real-time strategy game play and cognitive performance: a pilot study. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1–23.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0021-8.
  16. de Leeuw, J. R. (2015). jsPsych: a JavaScript library for creating behavioral experiments in a web browser. Behavior Research Methods, 47(1), 1–12.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0458-y.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Demanet, J., Verbruggen, F., Liefooghe, B., & Vandierendonck, A. (2010). Voluntary task switching under load: contribution of top-down and bottom-up factors in goal-directed behavior. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17(3), 387–393.  https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.3.387.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Dobrowolski, P., Hanusz, K., Sobczyk, B., Skorko, M., & Wiatrow, A. (2015). Cognitive enhancement in video game players: the role of video game genre. Computers in Human Behavior, 44, 59–63.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Elo, A. E. (1978). The rating of chess players, past and present. New York: Arco.Google Scholar
  21. Fröber, K., & Dreisbach, G. (2017). Keep flexible—keep switching! The influence of forced task switching on voluntary task switching. Cognition, 162, 48–53.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.024.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Garner, K. G., Tombu, M. N., & Dux, P. E. (2014). The influence of training on the attentional blink and psychological refractory period. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(4), 979–999.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0638-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Glass, B. D., Maddox, W. T., & Love, B. C. (2013). Real-time strategy game training: emergence of a cognitive flexibility trait. PLoS One, 8(8), e70350.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0070350.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Gnambs, T., & Appel, M. (2017). Is computer gaming associated with cognitive abilities? A population study among German adolescents. Intelligence, 61, 19–28.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.12.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gozli, D. G. (2017). Behaviour versus performance: the veiled commitment of experimental psychology. Theory & Psychology, 27(6), 741–758.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354317728130.
  26. Gozli, D. G., & Deng, W. (2017). Building blocks of psychology: on remaking the unkept promises of early schools. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 1–24.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-017-9405-7.
  27. Gozli, D. G., Bavelier, D., & Pratt, J. (2014). The effect of action video game playing on sensorimotor learning: evidence from a movement tracking task. Human Movement Science, 38, 152–162.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2014.09.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Green, C. S. (2014). The perceptual and cognitive effects of action video game experience. In F. C. Blumberg (Ed.), Learning by playing: video gaming in education (pp. 29–41). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature, 423(6939), 534–537.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01647.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2012). Learning, attentional control, and action video games. Current Biology, 22(6), R197–R206.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.012.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2015). Action video game training for cognitive enhancement. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 4, 103–108.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.04.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Green, C. S., Sugarman, M. A., Medford, K., Klobusicky, E., & Bavelier, D. (2012). The effect of action video game experience on task-switching. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(3), 984–994.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.12.020.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Green, C. S., Kattner, F., Eichenbaum, A., Bediou, B., Adams, D. M., Mayer, R. E., & Bavelier, D. (2017). Playing some video games but not others is related to cognitive abilities: a critique of Unsworth et al. (2015). Psychological Science, 28(5), 679–682.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616644837.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hvattum, L. M., & Arntzen, H. (2010). Using ELO ratings for match result prediction in association football. International Journal of Forecasting, 26(3), 460–470.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijforecast.2009.10.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd ed.). Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
  36. Karle, J. W., Watter, S., & Shedden, J. M. (2010). Task switching in video game players: benefits of selective attention but not resistance to proactive interference. Acta Psychologica, 134(1), 70–78.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.12.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—a review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019842.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Kim, Y.-H., Kang, D.-W., Kim, D., Kim, H.-J., Sasaki, Y., & Watanabe, T. (2015). Real-time strategy video game experience and visual perceptual learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(29), 10485–10492.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-14.2015.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Kray, J., Karbach, J., Haenig, S., & Freitag, C. (2012). Can task-switching training enhance executive control functioning in children with attention deficit/-hyperactivity disorder? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5(180).  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00180.
  40. Latham, A. J., Patston, L. L. M., & Tippett, L. J. (2013). The virtual brain: 30 years of video-game play and cognitive abilities. Frontiers in Psychology, 4.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00629.
  41. Linn, D. (2015). November 2015—Fallout 4 and Black Ops III arise while StarCraft II shines [Blog post]. Retrieved from http://caas.raptr.com/most-played-games-november-2015-fallout-4-and-black-ops-iii-arise-while-starcraft-ii-shines/
  42. Mayr, U., & Bell, T. (2006). On how to be unpredictable: evidence from the voluntary task-switching paradigm. Psychological Science, 17(9), 774–780.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01781.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1423–1442.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1423.Google Scholar
  44. Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Moradzadeh, L., Blumenthal, G., & Wiseheart, M. (2015). Musical training, bilingualism, and executive function: a closer look at task switching and dual-task performance. Cognitive Science, 39(5), 992–1020.  https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12183.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., & Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1425–1433.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611416999.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Neumann, C., Duboscq, J., Dubuc, C., Ginting, A., Irwan, A. M., Agil, M., et al. (2011). Assessing dominance hierarchies: validation and advantages of progressive evaluation with Elo-rating. Animal Behaviour, 82(4), 911–921.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Olfers, K. J. F., & Band, G. P. H. (2017). Game-based training of flexibility and attention improves task-switch performance: near and far transfer of cognitive training in an EEG study. Psychological Research.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0933-z.
  49. Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(37), 15583–15587.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0903620106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 220–244.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 41(1), 19–45.  https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748908402351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2013). Confidence intervals for two sample means: calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 74–80.  https://doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0133-x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Pohl, C., Kunde, W., Ganz, T., Conzelmann, A., Pauli, P., & Kiesel, A. (2014). Gaming to see: action video gaming is associated with enhanced processing of masked stimuli. Frontiers in Psychology, 5.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00070
  54. Powers, K. L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., Palladino, M. A., & Alfieri, L. (2013). Effects of video-game play on information processing: a meta-analytic investigation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(6), 1055–1079.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0418-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rubin, O., & Meiran, N. (2005). On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(6).Google Scholar
  57. Salminen, T., Strobach, T., Schubert, T., & Karbach, J. (2012). On the impacts of working memory training on executive functioning. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00166.
  58. Schwarz, K. A., Pfister, R., & Büchel, C. (2017). The being a patient effect: negative expectations based on group labeling and corresponding treatment affect patient performance. Psychology, Health & Medicine.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2017.1332375.
  59. Strobach, T., Frensch, P. A., & Schubert, T. (2012a). Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 13–24.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.02.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Strobach, T., Liepelt, R., Schubert, T., & Kiesel, A. (2012b). Task switching: effects of practice on switch and mixing costs. Psychological Research, 76(1), 74–83.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0323-x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Strobach, T., Salminen, T., Karbach, J., & Schubert, T. (2014). Practice-related optimization and transfer of executive functions: a general review and a specific realization of their mechanisms in dual tasks. Psychological Research, 78(6), 836–851.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0563-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Vandierendonck, A., Demanet, J., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2012). A chain-retrieval model for voluntary task switching. Cognitive Psychology, 65(2), 241–283.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.04.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology IIIUniversity of WürzburgWürzburgGermany

Personalised recommendations