Exams, tests, and essays are used in education systems worldwide to demonstrate students’ competencies, skills and knowledge (Fontaine et al. 2013; Stiggins 2009). Students’ academic results are an indication of their success and these results have social consequences: successful completion of a course, admission to a university program, winning a scholarship, earning a diploma and sometimes even finding employment. According to many authors, the pressure to succeed, using the metrics list above, can lead to cheating (Callahan 2004; Tchouata et al. 2014; McCabe et al. 2012; Lancaster and Clarke 2017). This phenomenon of cheating raises questions about the validity of grades and the credibility of diplomas awarded (Cizek 1999; Desalegn and Berhan 2014; Fendler et al. 2018). These validity and credibility questions are also relevant in the present context of COVID-19 pandemic which has suddenly imposed online teaching and assessments for teachers and students. To that effect, Corrigan-Gibbs et al., (Corrigan-Gibbs et al. 2015 p.28–29) point out that, with this new teaching and learning modality, “it will be crucial to preserve the same levels of trust, honesty, and integrity online that people expect from face-to-face interactions.”
Student cheating is not a new phenomenon. In fact, according to Fishman (2016), the first documented case of cheating, referring to the theft of an exam from a university printing office, has been reported in a research paper written by Barnes in 1904. Since then, the literature is replete with cases of cheating, albeit without a consensus about its prevalence. Some authors report that in general, as many as 80% of students are cheating (Qualls 2014) while others suggest numbers ranging from 30% to 60% (Bowers 1964; Jurdi et al. 2011; McCabe et al. 2001; Williams and Williams 2012). Statistics about cheating vary from one study to another, possibly because of differing definitions of cheating behaviors. For some authors, the statistics include cheating in written assignments (plagiarism) and on exams (Dodeen 2012) while others focus on cheating on exams only (Michaut 2013) or specifically, on plagiarism (Harper et al. 2019). And so, we observe a problem in the literature with the definitions of cheating that can include many or one type, the frequency of cheating and the generalization of statistics that refer to a mix of all these factors. Nonetheless, recent international research tend to confirm a high number of cases of cheating (Christensen Hughes and Mighty 2010; Ellahi et al. 2013; Fendler et al. 2018; Tchouata et al. 2014; Ma et al. 2008; McCabe et al. 2012; Stiles et al. 2017).
The phenomenon of cheating appears to be worldwide and at all levels from secondary school to university. Crittenden et al. (2009) studied the cheating culture within faculties of commerce in 36 countries. They define cheating culture as tolerance of cheating, beliefs in cheating and status of the cheating culture and targeted three specific predictors of cheating for their study: gender, level of corruption in the country, and socioeconomic environment. Their research showed that women are less likely to cheat than men, but also identified social factors that influence cheating, such as the level of corruption in the country and socioeconomic conditions. After illustrating the magnitude of the global phenomenon of cheating, they conclude that business students, who are future business leaders, appear to learn that results are more important than learning, and this, regardless of the way, ethical or not, they obtain these results (Crittenden et al. 2009). More recently, Miller et al. (2015) conducted a worldwide study of secondary school principals (35 countries) addressing cheating in secondary schools. They found that school principals within developing nations report more problematic cheating than school principals from wealthiest countries. They suggest that “schools in more economically disadvantaged nations may have more difficulty controlling undesired behaviors, such as cheating, among students” (p.226). Although there are few worldwide studies, large studies have been conducted in numerous countries (Michaut 2013; Harper et al. 2019; Denisova-Schmidt et al. 2019).
In Canada, Christensen Hughes and McCabe (2006) conducted a national study with 14,913 students from 11 universities that showed the prevalence of cheating, while indicating a higher proportion of self-reported cheating on exams (58%) and on written assignments (73%) in secondary school than at university level (respectively 18% and 53%). Although they did not survey secondary school directly, they asked first-year university students to reflect on their high school experience. Interestingly, their study also shows that students are sometimes confused about what constitutes serious cheating behavior. For example, collaborating with peers for a take-home exam is not perceived by students as serious cheating even though they know they are supposed to do it on their own as oppose to as a group. This confusion about the appropriateness, or not, of collaboration between students has been reported as well in Jurdi, Hage, and Chow’s study (Jurdi et al. 2011).
Exploring the literature on cheating on exams has led us to realize that while research in this area is abundant, there is a lack of Canadian research on this particular topic (Christensen Hughes and McCabe 2006; Jurdi et al. 2011; Wideman 2011) and most studies tend to simultaneously address both cheating on exams and plagiarism. Furthermore, few articles have reported on research addressing cheating on exams specifically by students in faculties of education (Bens 2010; Tchouata et al. 2014) and to our knowledge, no study has been conducted on this topic in a faculty of education of a Quebec university.
Among university students, those in the field of education will play a crucial role in the education of young people who will become the leaders of tomorrow. Preservice teachers in Quebec must develop 12 competencies, including competency number 12 “To demonstrate ethical and responsible professional behaviour in the performance of his or her duties” (Gouvernement du Québec 2001 p.55). Therefore, their teaching should reflect an ethical approach (Boon 2011; Jeffrey 2013; Jutras 2013) grounded on moral reasoning in their decision making (Cummings et al. 2007; Ndzedi 2016). These future teachers after graduation will assume a dual role as leaders in professional integrity and as models of integrity for their students (Boon 2011; Cummings et al. 2007). Taking stock of the phenomenon of cheating on exams in faculties of education thus becomes a preliminary yet essential step in any process aiming to ensure that future teachers are capable of assuming this dual role, especially in light of some research findings that demonstrate continuation of cheating in further studies and professional life (Christensen Hughes and McCabe 2006; Cronan et al. 2017; Ellahi et al. 2013; Novotney 2011).
Research purpose and questions
We aim to further the research on individual and contextual factors associated with cheating among university students enrolled in education programs. A better understanding of these factors may allow university administrators and professors to focus on strategies to reduce cheating and “create ethical organizations” (Van Yperen et al. 2011 p.5).
More precisely, our research objective is to examine students’ propensity to cheat on exams in the faculties of education at five francophone universities in the province of Quebec, Canada. This objective brings us to five specific questions:
-
1.
How big is the propensity to cheat on exams in faculties of education?
-
2.
Why do preservice teachers decide to cheat on exams?
-
3.
What methods are considered the best to cheat on exams?
-
4.
What methods do students used to cheat?
-
5.
What is the impact of specific individual factors, namely students’ academic goals, perception of control over tasks, engagement in studying, methods of cheating, and contextual factors, such as peers’ influence and institutional context, on the propensity to cheat?
Theoretical framework
Murdock and Anderman (2006) propose a theoretical model of cheating that includes three components: students’ goals, their expectations regarding achieving these goals, and their evaluation of the cost to achieve these goals. As illustrated in Fig. 1, their model provides an excellent overview of all factors related to propensity to cheat in the literature.
In our research, we focused on seven factors included in this theoretical model: six independent factors and one dependent factor (propensity to cheat). Four of these factors are related to the individual, namely the type of academic goals students aim to achieve in their program of study, students’ engagement in their studies, choice of method of cheating (in order to explore students’ perception and use of various methods to cheat), and students’ perception of control over the tasks to be achieved. These factors fall under two questions articulated in Murdock and Anderman’s model, namely what is my purpose? and can I do it? We were also interested in two factors embedded in contextual influence – peers’ influence and institutional policy or code on integrity. These two factors fall under the question What are the costs? Murdock and Anderman (2006) demonstrate that when students focus on performance goals more than on mastery goals, when they have poor expectations of their abilities to achieve these goals despite their efforts, and when they “assess that potential costs incurred from cheating are minimal, they are more apt to engage in dishonest behaviors” (p.130). These factors have also been studied by numerous authors although not specifically in education faculties (Bernardi et al. 2008; Denisova-Schmidt et al. 2019; Ellahi et al. 2013; Meng et al. 2014; Rinn et al. 2014; Tas and Tekkaya 2010).
Propensity to cheat on exams
It appears necessary to distinguish between cheating and plagiarism, two practices often studied together by researchers. Indeed, the literature on cheating usually includes cheating in written assignments, also called plagiarism, and cheating on exams. Plagiarism involves copying words or using a slightly modified text of an author without citation (Shei 2005; Walker 2010) in a written task where originality is expected (Fishman 2009). Cheating on exams, on the other hand, is a fraud committed by a student to increase the chances of success at examination (Chaput de Saintonge and Pavlovic 2004; Michaut 2013; Pavlin-Bernardić et al. 2017). This later definition was retained for the purpose of our study which focuses specifically on the propensity to cheat on exams.
Students’ academic goals
Murdock and Anderman (2006) associate performance goals with extrinsic motivation, mainly externally influenced (school, peers), while mastery goals are intrinsic and influenced by personal goals. Both types of goals are academic goals. Performance goals reflect a “desire to demonstrate skills, either by trying to be better than others, or by trying to avoid being less good than others” (Tchouata et al. 2014 p.49). Students who pursue performance goals are more concerned with comparing themselves with others while students who pursue mastery goals are engaged in tasks, are concerned with self-improvement, and are eager to learn and to integrate new knowledge (Anderman and Danner 2008). Many researchers also conclude that pursuing performance goals may lead to cheating. For example, studies conducted by Anderman and Midgley (2004), Murdock et al. (2007), and Tas and Tekkaya (2010) conclude that having performance goals, or being in a class where grades are valued more than learning, is a predictor of cheating. Similarly, Tchouata et al. (2014), Olafson et al. (2013) concluded that when students pursue academic goals mainly for the purpose of achieving high grades or obtaining a diploma rather than for learning, cheating could become an option. Other research also states that “results generally indicate that personal mastery goals are inversely related to cheating, whereas personal extrinsic goals are related positively to cheating” (Anderman and Danner 2008 p.167). Recently, Anderman and Won (2019) conducted a study to explore the relation between academic motivation (mastery, extrinsic or avoidance goals), personality variables (impulsivity and sensation-seeking) and cheating. Contrary to previous studies, these researchers concluded that the type of goals was not predictive of cheating. However, it was predictive of the student’s belief about the acceptability of cheating. Therefore, pursuing extrinsic or avoidance goals were associated with the belief that cheating is an acceptable behavior.
Students’ engagement in studying
Students’ engagement in their programs – as demonstrated by class attendance, amount of time spent studying, and procrastination in homework and study – have been studied as well (Ellahi et al. 2013; Guibert and Michaut 2009). Research has shown a link between a lack of engagement, manifested by poor class attendance and little time devoted to studying, and cheating (Ellahi et al. 2013), between partying and cheating (Whitley 1998), and between procrastinating and cheating (Patrzek et al. 2015). According to these researchers, students who are not very motivated by their studies, who devote little time to study and work, and who tend to procrastinate in their school tasks, are more likely to cheat. Students’ engagement might also be triggered by their perception of the relevance of what they learn. In her study, Bens (2010) suggests that students in teacher training programs may cheat when they do not see the applicability of the material they learn in classes. In other words, if it is not part of what they need in their real work as teachers, students may be more likely to cheat. Although these studies concur in associating the likelihood of cheating with a lack of engagement, others report that “surprisingly, students’ motivations toward reading, writing, and learning do not seem to have a valuable impact on the likelihood of their misconduct” (Grira and Jaeck 2019).
Perception of control over tasks
Students’ perceptions of the degree of control they exercise over their school activity has also been studied by some researchers (Rettinger and Kramer 2009; Whitley 1998). The degree of control perceived by the student refers to the concept of “locus of control” associated with attribution theory (Weiner 1986). Despite the fact that the influence of the perception of one’s control over a task is not always clearly defined in the literature, researchers suggest some link between the perception of having little control over tasks and cheating (Rettinger and Kramer 2009; Whitley 1998). Similarly, Rinn et al. (2014) found a low to medium correlation between students’ perceptions of their ability to perform the task and a propensity to cheat.
Methods of cheating
Cizek (1999) and Faucher and Caves (2009) propose the following three categories of methods to cheat. One category is using forbidden material during exams, for example notes on pieces of paper inside or outside the classroom or notes written on one’s own body. A second is taking, receiving or giving information about the exam that should not be shared. Examples of this second category include glancing at a peer’s copy of an exam, exchanging exams, and using sign language or codes for communicating answers. According to Bernardi et al. (2008), this latter method of cheating is facilitated when exams are composed of multiple-choice and true or false questions. Another method falling in this category is the reception by students of information about an exam from students of another cohort that has already written the same exam. By way of remedies, Christensen Hughes and McCabe (2006) suggest that “faculty may need to either substantially change their exams between semesters” or “for courses with multiple sections … create different exams or have all students write the same exam at the same time” (p.15). The third category of cheating methods is called “circumventing the process of assessment” (Faucher and Caves 2009 p.38). This category includes all excuses that students can provide to avoid the exam on the appointed day. Excuses such as sudden illness or the death of a grandmother (again!) are good examples. It is also worth noting that the development of technology opens the door to a wide range of new high-tech devices such as smartphones, smart watches, or earpieces can facilitate cheating on exams (Michaut 2013).
Institutional context
Other reasons put forward for cheating are related to the institutional context. Most academic institutions have legal frameworks, policies, and regulations to guide practices related to student assessment. Whether it is a policy on academic integrity, a policy on plagiarism, or a code of honor, these documents are accessible to students and often reproduced, in part or in full, in the course outlines. While the literature clearly indicates a decrease in cheating associated with the existence of institutional policy on integrity or code of honor and cheating (McCabe et al. 2012; McCabe and Trevino 1993), it also stresses the importance of having consequences of cheating known and enforced in order to reduce cheating (Meng et al. 2014; Murdock and Anderman 2006; Schuhmann et al. 2013). In the context of online teaching, it seems that having a code of honor is far less effective than having stern warnings about the consequences of cheating while doing the exam (Corrigan-Gibbs et al. 2015). Yet, researchers who have studied the influence of these legal frameworks on the phenomenon of cheating conclude that, in many cases, students do not know about or understand these documents (Ellahi et al. 2013; Ma et al. 2008). They are unaware of the consequences associated with cheating and consider the risks to be low (Murdock and Anderman 2006; Ma et al. 2008; Meng et al. 2014; Schuhmann et al. 2013).
Peers’ influence
As for institutional context, the behavior of peers is included in the question What are the costs? in Murdock and Adderman’s theoretical model (Murdock and Anderman 2006). The influence of peers on a student’s decision to cheat has been widely documented since the earliest studies on cheating (Bowers 1964; Crittenden et al. 2009; Cummings et al. 2002; Christensen Hughes and McCabe 2006; Ellahi et al. 2013; Kisamore et al. 2007; Ma et al. 2008; McCabe and Trevino 1997; Meng et al. 2014; Rettinger and Kramer 2009; Whitley 1998). Although there is consensus that peers’ influence exists, the extent of this influence is unclear. Some authors suggest that when students are competing against one another, thereby creating a competitive environment, it appears to lead to cheating (Cizek 1999; Whitley 1998). However, a directly competitive environment doesn’t seem to be necessary for students to cheat. In fact, mere knowledge that one’s peers are cheating would be sufficient to enhance the chance of a fellow student cheating as well. Students tend to “go with the flow” and justify their behavior by pointing out the commonness of the cheating practice (Crittenden et al. 2009; McCabe and Trevino 1997; Meng et al. 2014; Schuhmann et al. 2013). In that sense, Rettinger and Kramer (2009) argue that simply knowing that student friends are cheating, therefore being immersed in a “cheating culture”, and possibly leading to “peer pressure”, is enough to motivate a student to cheat. They argue that “knowing people who cheat (or have cheated) is a risk factor for starting to cheat” (p.296). From their study conducted with 600 university students in Ukraine, Denisova-Schmidt et al. (2019) report that the perception of the action of cheating, which they labeled “corruption”, from others, including peers in the classroom but also teachers, politicians, and relatives, would be sufficient for students to engage in cheating behaviors. In this respect, researchers (Meng et al. 2014; Pavlin-Bernardić et al. 2017; Smith et al. 2012) argue that the cheating student resorts to ‘neutralization’ techniques to justify his action (Sykes and Matza 1957). Thus, cheating students deny their responsibility and convince themselves that they do no harm to anyone, or that the victim (the teacher giving the exam) has deserved it or that everybody does it as well. These “neutralizing attitudes are positively correlated with student cheating” (Rettinger and Kramer 2009 p.295). Finally, the opinion of students and their entourage about the acceptability of cheating is linked to the decision to cheat (Meng et al. 2014; Crittenden et al. 2009; Schuhmann et al. 2013).
In summary, factors influencing students in their decision to cheat could be associated with each student and labeled “individual influences” (Schuhmann et al. 2013 p.9). They encompass students’ background characteristics, attitudes, perceptions, etc. (Yu et al. 2017). Other factors are related to the context (policy on integrity, code of honor, peers’ behaviors, etc.) and associated with the institution students are enrolled at (Christensen Hughes and Mighty 2010; Ellahi et al. 2013; McCabe et al. 2012).