Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Ensemble Hypothesis of Human Cognitive Evolution

  • Theoretical Article
  • Published:
Evolutionary Psychological Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Darwinian view is that human cognition is continuous with non-human cognition. Comparative mental abilities differ quantitatively but not qualitatively. A predominant alternative hypothesis is that language qualitatively separates human beings and other primates. The ensemble hypothesis examined here suggests that language is but one of five human capacities that produce a qualitatively unique form of cognition because of their interactions with each other. Each one taken separately has a likely precursor in non-human cognition, but taken together, their effects on cognition are not additive but interactive. The human mental ensemble proposed here includes an advanced executive attention component of working memory, a theory of mind augmenting social cognition, language, the ability to interpret information using inner speech and causal inference, and an episodic memory capable of mental time travel. Only in the evolution of Homo sapiens did these five components come together to enable non-linear changes in cognition on multiple fronts, resulting in the emergent form of cognition we experientially know and scientifically study today. Empirical evidence from behavioral studies, lesion studies, and studies involving neuroatypical populations provide tentative support for the hypothesis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, T. A., & Fortin, N. J. (2013). The evolution of episodic memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 110, 10379–10386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: theories, models and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldo, J. V., Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D., Ludy, C., Raskin, P., & Kim, J. (2005). Is problem solving dependent on language? Brain and Language, 92, 240–250.

  • Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Yosef, O. (2002). The upper Paleolithic revolution. Annual Review of Anthropology, 31, 363–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi, U., Järvinen-Pasley, Doyle, T. F., Reilly, J., Reiss, A. L., & Korenberg, J. R. (2007). Affect, social behavior, and the brain in Williams syndrome. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 99–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (1993). Aspects of transmission of tool-use in wild chimpanzees. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language, and cognition in human evolution (pp. 171–183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caspari, I., Parkinson, S. R., LaPointe, L. L., & Katz, R. C. (1998). Working memory and aphasia. Brain and Cognition, 37, 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2001). Executive functions of the frontal lobes and the evolutionary ascendancy of Homo sapiens. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11, 255–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2005). Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 15, 5–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2007). The working memory account of Neandertal cognition—how phonological storage may be related to recursion and pragmatics in modern speech. Journal of Human Evolution, 52, 707–710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2016). An introduction to cognitive archaeology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 386–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2012). The wandering mind: mental time travel, theory of mind, and language. Análise Social, 205, 870–893.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cragg, L., & Nation, K. (2010). Language and the development of cognitive control. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 631–642.

  • d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Lawson, G., Vanhaeren, M., Tillier, A. M., Soressi, M., et al. (2003). Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism, and music—an alternative multidisciplinary perspective. Journal of World Prehistory, 17, 1–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1905). (1871, 1st ed.). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex (2) New York: Collier.

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task-switching: a dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 148–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2009). Comparative social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 87–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagöt, J., & de Lillo, C. (2011). A comparative study of working memory: immediate serial spatial recall in baboons (Papio papio) and humans. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3870–3880.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fouts, R. S., Fouts, D. H., & Schoenfeld, D. (1984). Sign language conversational interactions among chimpanzees. Sign Language Studies, 42, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T., & Van Cantfort, T. E. (Eds.). (1989). Teaching sign language to chimpanzees. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain, 123, 1293–1326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1997). The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning. NeuroReport, 8, 1305–1310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (2002). The structure of evolutionary theory. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B. (2007). From nonhuman to human mind: what changed and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 60–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasselhorn, M., Mähler, C., & Grube, D. (2005). Theory of mind, working memory, and verbal ability in preschool children: the proposal of a relay race model of the developmental dependencies. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 219–237). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Yang, C., Berwick, R. C., Tattersall, I., Ryan, M. J., Watumull, J., Chomsky, N., & Lewontin, R. C. (2014). The mystery of language evolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 401, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffecker, J. F., & Hoffecker, I. T. (2017). Technological complexity and the global dispersion of modern humans. Evolutionary Anthropology, 26, 285–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, L., & Low, J. (2010). Do children with autism use inner speech and visuospatial resources for the service of executive control? Evidence from suppression in dual tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 369–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. A. (2009). The fruit, the tree, and the serpent: Why we see so well. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly, J., Lincoln, A., & Adolphs, R. (2000). Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Supplement), 30–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, R. T. (2013). The making of the mind: The neuroscience of human nature. Amherst: Prometheus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6, 449–476.

  • Klein, R. G., & Edgar, B. (2002). The dawn of human culture. New York: Nevraumont Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurczek, J., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Duff, M. (2013). Hippocampal contributions to language: evidence of referential processing deficits in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1346–1354.

  • Lee, C. S., & Therriault, D. J. (2013). The cognitive underpinnings of creative thought: a latent variable analysis exploring the roles of intelligence and working memory in three creative thinking processes. Intelligence, 41, 306–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Losh, M., Bellugi, U., Reilly, J., & Anderson, D. (2000). Narrative as a social engagement tool: the excessive use of evaluation in narratives from children with Williams syndrome. Narrative Inquiry, 10, 265–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G., James, L. E., & Hadley, C. B. (2008). Amnesic H. M.’s performance on the language competence test: parallel deficits in memory and sentence production. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 280–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G., James, L. E., Hadley, C. B., & Fogler, K. A. (2011). Speech errors of amnesic H. M.: unlike everyday slips of the tongue. Cortex, 47, 377–408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G., Johnson, W. W., Fazel, V., & James, L. E. (2013). Compensating for language deficits in amnesia I: H.M.’s spared retrieval categories. Brain Sciences, 3, 262–293.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, R. C. (1987). Articulatory and phonological deficits in short-term memory and their relation to syntactic processing. Brain and Language, 32, 159–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty, S., & Brooks, A. S. (2002). The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39, 453–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H. L. (1968). Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: a 14-year follow-up study of H. M. Neuropsychologia, 6, 215–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J. C. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 115, 123–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moscovitch, M., Cabeza, R., Winocur, G., & Nadel, L. (2016). Episodic memory and beyond: the hippocampus and neocortex in transformation. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 105–134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nashida, T., & Hiraiwa, M. (1982). Natural history of a tool-using behavior by wild chimpanzees feeding on wood-boring ants. Journal of Human Evolution, 11, 73–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newton, M. A., & Villiers, J. G. (2007). Thinking while talking: adults fail nonverbal false-belief reasoning. Psychological Science, 18, 574–579.

  • Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., & Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 46–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 115, 123–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. PNAS, 107, 8893–8999.

  • Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition, 95, 201–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Read, D. W. (2008). Working memory: a cognitive limit to non-human primate recursive thinking prior to hominid evolution. Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 676–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reverberi, C., Shallice, T., D’Agnostini, S., Skrap, M., & Bonatti, L. L. (2009). Cortical bases of deductive reasoning: inference, memory, and metadeduction. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1107–1116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, W. A. (2002). Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin, 128, 473–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogalsky, C., Matchin, W., & Hickok, G. (2008). Broca’s area, sentence comprehension, and working memory: an fMRI study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, Article 14, 1-13.

  • Roser, M. E., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2004). Automatic brains—interpretive minds. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 56–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roser, M. E., Fugelsan, J. A., Dunbar, K. N., Corballis, P. M., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). Dissociating processes supporting causal perception and causal inference in the brain. Neuropsychology, 19, 591–602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). The emergence of language. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language, and cognition in human evolution (pp. 86–108). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Springer, S. P., & Deutsch, G. (1998). Left brain right brain: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (5th ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S., & Metcalfe, J. (Eds.). (2005). The missing link in cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. & Herrmann, E. (2010). Ape and human cognition. What's the difference? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 3–8.

  • Trask, L., Tobias, P. V., Wynn, T., Davidson, I., Noble, W., & Mellars, P. (1998). The origins of speech. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 8, 69–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Urbanski, M., Bréchemier, M. L., Garcin, B., Bendetowicz, D., de Schotten, M. T., et al. (2016). Reasoning by analogy requires the left frontal pole: lesion mapping and clinical implications. Brain, 139, 1783–1799.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallar, G., & Baddeley, A. D. (1984). Phonological short term store: phonological processing and sentence comprehension: a neuropsychological case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 121–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.) Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Wadley, L. (2013). Recognizing complex cognition through innovative technology in Stone Age and Paleolithic sites. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 23, 163–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J., Mayberry, M. T., & Durkin, K. (2006). Inner speech impairments in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 857–865.

  • Wolford, G., Miller, M. B., & Gazzaniga, M. (2000). The left hemisphere’s role in hypothesis formation. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(6), RC64 1–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, T., & Coolidge, F. L. (2011). The implications of the working memory model for the evolution of modern cognition. International Journal of Evolutionary Biologyhttps://doi.org/10.4061/2011/741357.

  • Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11, 37–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Victoria Litvinova for her assistance in conducting our literature search. We also thank Thomas Wynn and another anonymous reviewer for their helpful critiques of an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ronald T. Kellogg.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kellogg, R.T., Evans, L. The Ensemble Hypothesis of Human Cognitive Evolution. Evolutionary Psychological Science 5, 1–12 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-018-0159-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-018-0159-3

Keywords

Navigation