Execution, Violent Punishment and Selection for Religiousness in Medieval England

Theoretical Article

Abstract

Frost and Harpending, Evolutionary Psychology, 13 (2015), have argued that the increasing use of capital punishment across the Middle Ages in Europe altered the genotype, helping to create a less violent and generally more law-abiding population. Developing this insight, we hypothesise that the same system of violent punishments would also have helped to genotypically create a more religious society by indirectly selecting for religiousness, through the execution of men who had not yet sired any offspring. We estimate the selection differential for religiousness based on genetic correlation data for conceivably related traits, and compare that to the actual increase in religiosity across the Middle Ages. We further explore other mechanisms by which religiousness was being selected for in Medieval England, and conclude that executions most likely contributed substantially to the increase in religiosity, but that other selection pressures also played a role.

Keywords

Religion Selection Execution Medieval 

References

  1. Adam, R. (1998). The Abuses of Punishment. London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
  2. Artello, K., & Williams, S. (2014). Stress and crime. In The Wiley encyclopedia of health, illness, behavior and society. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  3. Blackstone, W. (1765). Commentaries on the Laws of England. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  4. Blume, M. (2009). The reproductive benefits of religious affiliation. In E. Voland & W. Schiefenhövel (Eds.), The biological evolution of religious mind and behavior. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  5. Bowen, J. (1975). A history of western education: civilization of Europe, sixth to sixteenth century (Vol. 2). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  6. Caldwell-Harris, C., Fox Murphy, C., Velazquez, T., & McNamara, P. (2011). Religious belief systems of persons with high functioning autism. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society, 3362–3366. http://csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu/proceedings/2011/papers/0782/paper0782.pdf.
  7. Chakrabarti, B., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2013). Understanding the genetics of empathy and the autistic spectrum. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & M. Lombardo (Eds.), Understanding other minds: perspectives from developmental social neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Clark, G. (2007). A farewell to alms: a brief economic history of the world. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Clark, J. (2002). The religious orders in pre-reformation England. In J. Clark (Ed.), The religious orders in pre-Reformation England. Woodbridge: The Boydell Press.Google Scholar
  10. Dodds, B. (2008). Patterns of decline: arable production in England, France and Castile, 1370–1450. In B. Dodds & R. Britnell (Eds.), Agriculture and rural society after the black death: common themes and regional variations. Hatfield: University of Hertfordshire Press.Google Scholar
  11. Dunkel, C., Reeve, C., Woodley of Menie, M. A., & Van der Linden, D. (2015). A comparative study of the general factor of personality in Jewish and non-Jewish populations. Personality and Individual Differences, 78, 63–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dutton, E. & Van der Linden, D. (2017). Why is intelligence negatively associated with religiousness? Evolutionary Psychological Science. doi:10.1007/s40806-017-0101-0.
  13. Dutton, E., Van der Linden, D., & Lynn, R. (2016a). The negative Flynn Effect: a systematic literature review. Intelligence, 59, 163–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dutton, E., Madison, G., & Lynn, R. (2016b). Demographic, economic, and genetic factors related to national differences in ethnocentric attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 137–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dutton, E., & Charlton, B. (2015). The genius famine: why we need geniuses, why they’re dying out and why we must rescue them. Buckingham: University of Buckingham Press.Google Scholar
  16. Ellis, L., Hoskin, A., Dutton, E., & Nyborg, H. (2017). The future of secularism: a biologically informed theory supplemented with cross-cultural evidence. Evolutionary Psychological Science. doi:10.1007/s40806-017-0090-z.
  17. Figueredo, A., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B., et al. (2006). Consilience and life history theory: from genes to brain to reproductive strategy. Developmental Review, 26, 243–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Frost, P. & Harpending, H. (2015). Western Europe, state formation, and genetic pacification. Evolutionary Psychology, 13, doi:10.1177/147470491501300114.
  19. Gibson, I. (1978). The English vice: beating, sex and shame in Victorian England and after. London, England: Duckworth.Google Scholar
  20. Gregory-Abbott, C. (2016). Sacred outlaws: outlawry and the medieval church. In J. Appleby (Ed.), Outlaws in Medieval and early Modern England: crime, government and society, C.1066–c.1600. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  21. Hammond, R., & Axelrod, R. (2006). The evolution of ethnocentric behaviour. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 50, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hatcher, J., & Bailey, M. (2001). Modelling the middle ages: the history and theory of England’s economic development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Harper-Bill, C. (1991). Editor’s introduction. In C. Harper-Bill (Ed.), Religious belief and ecclesiastical careers in late Medieval England. Woodbridge: The Boydell Press.Google Scholar
  24. Horsburgh, V. A., Schermer, J. A., Veselka, L., & Vernon, P. A. (2009). A behavioural genetic study of mental toughness and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 100–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Im, D. (2016). Template to perpetrate: an update on violence in autism spectrum disorder. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 24, 14–35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Jensen, A. (1998). The g factor: the science of mental ability. Westport: Praeger.Google Scholar
  27. Jones, T., Dolan, T., Dor, J., Fletcher, A., & Yeager, R. (2003). Who murdered Geoffrey Chaucer: a medieval mystery. New York: St Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
  28. Kay, A. C., Shepherd, S., Blatz, C. W., et al. (2010). For God (or) country: the hydraulic relation between government instability and belief in religious sources of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 725–739.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Knafo, A., Israel, S., & Ebstein, R. (2011). Heritability of children’s prosocial behavior and differential susceptibility to parenting by variation in the dopamine receptor D4 gene. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 53–67.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Koenig, H. (2012). Religion, spirituality, and health: the research and clinical implications. ISRN Psychiatry. doi:10.5402/2012/278730.
  31. Koenig, H., McGue, M., Krueger, R., & Bouchard, T. (2005). Genetic and environmental influences on religiousness: findings for retrospective and current religiousness ratings. Journal of Personality, 73, 471–478.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Lewis, G. J., & Bates, T. (2013). Common genetic influences underpin religiosity, community integration, and existential uncertainty. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 398–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lynn, R. (2011). Dysgenics: genetic deterioration in modern populations (Second ed.). London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.Google Scholar
  34. McCoy, M., & Keen, S. (2013). Child abuse and neglect. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  35. McSheffrey, S., & Tanner, R. (2003). Lollards of Coventry, 1486–1522. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Nettle, D. (2007). Personality: what makes you who you are. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Newberg, A., D’Aquili, E., & Rause, V. (2002). Why God won’t go away: brain science and the biology of belief. New York: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
  38. Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious pro-sociality. Science, 322, 58–62.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Orme, N. (2006). Medieval schools: from Roman Britain to Renaissance England. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Ridley, J. (1988). The Tudor age. London: Constable.Google Scholar
  41. Ryrie, A. (2017). The age of reformation: the Tudor and Stewart realms 1485–1603 (Second ed.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  42. Saroglou, V. (2002). Religion and the five factors of personality: a meta-analytic review. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 15–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sela, Y., Shackelford, T., & Liddle, J. (2015). When religion makes it worse: religiously motivated violence as a sexual selection weapon. In D. Sloane & J. Van Slyke (Eds.), The attraction of religion: a new evolutionary psychology of religion. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
  44. Tick, J., Bolton, P., Happe, F., Rutter, M., & Rijsdijk, F. (2016). Heritability of autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of twin studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Applied Disciplines, 57, 585–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Tremlin, T. (2012). The origins of religion. In P. McNamara & W. Wildman (Eds.), Science and the worlds’ religions: volume I: origins and destinies. Santa Barbara: Praeger.Google Scholar
  46. Vaas, R. (2009). God, gains and genes. In E. Voland & W. Schiefenhövel (Eds.), The biological evolution of religious mind and behavior. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  47. Van der Linden, D., Dunkel, C., Beaver, K., & Louwen, M. (2015). The unusual suspect: the general factor of personality (GFP), life history theory, and delinquent behavior. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences. doi:10.1037/ebs0000027.
  48. Vernon, P. A., Villani, V. C., Schermer, J. A., & Petrides, K. V. (2008). Phenotypic and genetic associations between the Big Five and trait emotional intelligence. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 11, 524–530.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Willemsen, A. (2008). Back to the schoolyard: the daily practice of medieval and Renaissance education. Bristol: Isd.Google Scholar
  50. Wilson, D. (2002). Darwin’s cathedral. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: towards the unity of knowledge. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
  52. Woodley, M. A., & Figueredo, A. J. (2013). Historical variability in heritable general intelligence: it’s evolutionary origins and sociocultural consequences. Buckingham: University of Buckingham Press.Google Scholar
  53. Wrigley, E., & Schofield, R. (1989). The population history of England, 1541–1871. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Ulster Institute for Social ResearchLondonUK
  2. 2.University of UmeåUmeåSweden

Personalised recommendations