Abstract
Objectives and Method
Anecdotal reports indicate more severe psychological distress following technological catastrophes in comparison to natural disasters. Previous research also suggests a more negative evaluation of the outcomes of disasters if they are manmade. On the other hand, evolutionary neuroscience shows differential neural processing of ancient and modern threats. Building upon this literature, we probed valence and arousal ratings of stimuli depicting natural and technological disasters in several standardized affective stimuli datasets used in neuroscience and psychological research.
Results
Our results show that while technological disasters are rated as slightly less arousing than natural disasters they are rated as significantly more unpleasant.
Conclusion
It seems the evolutionary age of disasters is one of the factors that affect emotional experiences evoked by these threats and can impact our evaluations of catastrophes. We discuss how evolutionary psychology might explain our findings and help us to better understand the biological and learned roots of our biases in risk perception.
Similar content being viewed by others
Data Availability
All data generated and analyzed during this study are included in this published article.
References
Ahad, N. A., & Yahaya, S. S. S. (2014, July). Sensitivity analysis of Welch’s t-test. In AIP Conference proceedings (Vol. 1605, No. 1, pp. 888–893). American Institute of Physics.
Apicella, C., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2020). Beyond WEIRD: A review of the last decade and a look ahead to the global laboratory of the future. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 319–329.
Baum, A., Fleming, R., & Davidson, L., M (1983). Natural Disaster and Technological catastrophe. Environment and Behavior, 15(3), 333–354. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916583153004.
Behrensmeyer, A. K. (2006). Climate change and human evolution. Science, 311(5760), 476–478.
Blanchette, I. (2006). Snakes, spiders, guns, and syringes: How specific are evolutionary constraints on the detection of threatening stimuli? Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 59(8), 1484–1504.
Blumenthal, A., Caparos, S., & Blanchette, I. (2022). How threat shapes attention and memory in the Himba, a remote people of Namibia. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 00220221231175063.
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59.
Brown, C., El-Deredy, W., & Blanchette, I. (2010). Attentional modulation of visual-evoked potentials by threat: Investigating the effect of evolutionary relevance. Brain and cognition, 74(3), 281–287.
Brun, W. (1992). Cognitive components in risk perception: Natural versus manmade risks. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5(2), 117–132. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.3960050204.
Cao, Z., Zhao, Y., Tan, T., Chen, G., Ning, X., Zhan, L., & Yang, J. (2014). Distinct brain activity in processing negative pictures of animals and objects—The role of human contexts. Neuroimage, 84, 901–910. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.064.
Carretié, L., Tapia, M., López-Martín, S., & Albert, J. (2019). EmoMadrid: An emotional pictures database for affect research. Motivation and Emotion, 43(6), 929–939.
Center for research on the Epidemiology of Disasters (2022, December 05). EM-DAT Public. https://public.emdat.be/.
Center for research on the Epidemiology of Disasters (2022, September 20). General Classification. https://www.emdat.be/classification.
Charles, S. T., & Almeida, D. M. (2006). Daily reports of symptoms and negative affect: Not all symptoms are the same. Psychology and Health, 21(1), 1–17.
Cinq-Mars, J., Blumenthal, A., Grund, A., Hétu, S., & Blanchette, I. (2022). DLPFC controls the rapid neural response to visual threat: An ERP and rTMS study. Brain Research, 1784, 147850.
Colibazzi, T., Posner, J., Wang, Z., Gorman, D., Gerber, A., Yu, S., & Peterson, B. S. (2010). Neural systems subserving valence and arousal during the experience of induced emotions. Emotion, 10(3), 377.
Crone, D. L., Bode, S., Murawski, C., & Laham, S. M. (2018). The Socio-Moral Image Database (SMID): A novel stimulus set for the study of social, moral and affective processes. PLoS One, 13(1), e0190954.
Delacre, M., Lakens, D., & Leys, C. (2017). Why psychologists should by default use Welch’s t-test instead of Student’s t-test. International Review of Social Psychology, 30(1).
Dhum, M., Herwig, U., Opialla, S., Siegrist, M., & Brühl, A. B. (2017). Evolutionary and modern image content differentially influence the processing of emotional pictures. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 11, 415.
Diconne, K., Kountouriotis, G. K., Paltoglou, A. E., Parker, A., & Hostler, T. J. (2022). Presenting KAPODI–The Searchable database of emotional Stimuli sets. Emotion Review, 14(1), 84–95.
Erlich, N., Lipp, O. V., & Slaughter, V. (2013). Of hissing snakes and angry voices: Human infants are differentially responsive to evolutionary fear-relevant sounds. Developmental science, 16(6), 894–904.
Fagerland, M. W., & Sandvik, L. (2009). The wilcoxon–mann–whitney test under scrutiny. Statistics in medicine, 28(10), 1487–1497.
Fang, Z., Li, H., Chen, G., & Yang, J. (2016). Unconscious processing of negative animals and objects: Role of the amygdala revealed by fMRI. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 10, 146.
Garland, B. (2016). The Risk of Energy Production. http://www.nuceng.ca/refer/risk/risk.htm.
Grattan, J., & Torrence, R. (2003). Natural disasters and cultural change. Routledge.
Grimm, A., Hulse, L., Preiss, M., & Schmidt, S. (2012). Post-and peritraumatic stress in disaster survivors: An explorative study about the influence of individual and event characteristics across different types of disasters. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 3(1), 7382.
Hart, A. (2001). Mann-Whitney test is not just a test of medians: Differences in spread can be important. Bmj, 323(7309), 391–393.
Hogarth, R. M., Portell, M., Cuxart, A., & Kolev, G. I. (2011). Emotion and reason in everyday risk perception. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 24(2), 202–222.
Hornsey, M. J., & Fielding, K. S. (2020). Understanding (and reducing) inaction on climate change. Social Issues and Policy Review, 14(1), 3–35.
Katsampouris, E., Turner-Cobb, J. M., Arnold, R., & Barnett, J. C. (2022a). a. unconscious associations between stressor type and ability to cope: An experimental approach using ancient and modern sources of stress. British Journal of Health Psychology, 27(3), 1011–1025.
Katsampouris, E., Turner-Cobb, J. M., Barnett, J. C., & Arnold, R. S. (2022b). b. can ancient and modern stressors be distinguished? A mixed-methods exploration of psychosocial characteristics and health symptoms in young and older adults. Journal of health psychology, 27(3), 624–636.
Kitagawa, K. (2021). Disaster risk reduction activities as learning. Natural hazards, 105(3), 3099–3118.
Kurdi, B., Lozano, S., & Banaji, M. R. (2017). Introducing the open affective standardized image set (OASIS). Behavior research methods, 49(2), 457–470.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 1(39–58), 3.
Leventhal, E. A., Hansell, S., Diefenbach, M., Leventhal, H., & Glass, D. C. (1996). Negative affect and self-report of physical symptoms: Two longitudinal studies of older adults. Health Psychology, 15(3), 193.
Levin, N. E. (2015). Environment and climate of early human evolution. Annual Review of Earth and Planetary Sciences, 43, 405–429.
Li, N. P., van Vugt, M., & Colarelli, S. M. (2018). The evolutionary mismatch hypothesis: Implications for psychological science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(1), 38–44.
LoBue, V. (2014). Deconstructing the snake: The relative roles of perception, cognition, and emotion on threat detection. Emotion, 14(4), 701.
LoBue, V., & Adolph, K. E. (2019). Fear in infancy: Lessons from snakes, spiders, heights, and strangers. Developmental psychology, 55(9), 1889.
Loschky, L. C., & Larson, A. M. (2010). The natural/man-made distinction is made before basic-level distinctions in scene gist processing. Visual Cognition, 18(4), 513–536.
Magalhães, S. D. S., Miranda, D. K., Miranda, D. M. D., Malloy-Diniz, L. F., & Romano-Silva, M. A. (2018). The Extreme Climate Event Database (EXCEED): Development of a picture database composed of drought and flood stimuli. PLoS One, 13(9), e0204093.
Morrice, S. (2013). Heartache and Hurricane Katrina: Recognising the influence of emotion in post-disaster return decisions. Area, 45(1), 33–39.
Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological review, 108(3), 483.
Oishi, S., & Komiya, A. (2017). Natural disaster risk and collectivism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 48(8), 1263–1270.
Oliva, A., & Torralba, A. (2001). Modeling the shape of the scene: A holistic representation of the spatial envelope. International journal of computer vision, 42(3), 145–175.
Pandis, N. (2016). Using linear regression for t tests and analysis of variance. American journal of orthodontics and dentofacial orthopedics, 149(5), 769.
Paradiso, S., Johnson, D. L., Andreasen, N. C., O’Leary, D. S., Watkins, G. L., Ponto, B., L. L., & Hichwa, R. D. (1999). Cerebral blood flow changes associated with attribution of emotional valence to pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral visual stimuli in a PET study of normal subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(10), 1618–1629.
Park, C. L. (2016). Meaning making in the context of disasters. Journal of clinical psychology, 72(12), 1234–1246.
Pisor, A. C., & Jones, J. H. (2021). Human adaptation to climate change: An introduction to the special issue. American Journal of Human Biology, 33(4), e23530.
Ribeiro, R. L., Pompéia, S., & Bueno, O. F. A. (2005). Comparison of brazilian and american norms for the international affective picture system (IAPS). Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry, 27, 208–215.
Schneider, C. R., Zaval, L., & Markowitz, E. M. (2021). Positive emotions and climate change. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 42, 114–120.
Scholz, C. A., Johnson, T. C., Cohen, A. S., King, J. W., Peck, J. A., Overpeck, J. T., & Pierson, J. (2007). East African megadroughts between 135 and 75 thousand years ago and bearing on early-modern human origins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(42), 16416–16421.
Schreier, A., & Evans, G. (2003). Adrenal cortical response of young children to modern and ancient stressors. Current Anthropology, 44(2), 306–309.
Seligman, M. E. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior therapy, 2(3), 307–320.
Shapouri, S., & Martin, L. L. (2021). Snakes vs. guns: A systematic review of comparisons between phylogenetic and ontogenetic threats. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-021-00181-5.
Shariff, A., Bonnefon, J. F., & Rahwan, I. (2017). Psychological roadblocks to the adoption of self-driving vehicles. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(10), 694–696.
Siegrist, M., & Sütterlin, B. (2014). Human and nature-caused hazards: The affect heuristic causes biased decisions. Risk Analysis, 34(8), 1482–1494. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12179.
Smith, A. (2022, January 24). B. 2021 U.S. billion-dollar weather and climate disasters in historical context. Climate.gov. https://www.climate.gov/news-features/blogs/beyond-data/2021-us-billion-dollar-weather-and-climate-disasters-historical.
Sonnberger, M., Ruddat, M., Arnold, A., Scheer, D., Poortinga, W., Böhm, G., & Tvinnereim, E. (2021). Climate concerned but anti-nuclear: Exploring (dis) approval of nuclear energy in four european countries. Energy Research & Social Science, 75, 102008.
Stanley, S. M. (1992). An ecological theory for the origin of Homo. Paleobiology, 18(3), 237–257.
Subra, B., Muller, D., Fourgassie, L., Chauvin, A., & Alexopoulos, T. (2018). Of guns and snakes: Testing a modern threat superiority effect. Cognition and emotion, 32(1), 81–91.
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2014). The parasite-stress theory of sociality, the behavioral immune system, and human social and cognitive uniqueness. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(4), 257.
Trauth, M. H., Maslin, M. A., Deino, A. L., Strecker, M. R., Bergner, A. G., & Dühnforth, M. (2007). High-and low-latitude forcing of Plio-Pleistocene East African climate and human evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 53(5), 475–486.
Weierich, M. R., Kleshchova, O., Rieder, J. K., & Reilly, D. M. (2019). The complex affective scene set (COMPASS): Solving the social content problem in affective visual stimulus sets. Collabra: Psychology, 5(1).
Zhang, S., & Guo, Q. (2019). Superiority effect in detecting phylogenetic threats under unattentional condition: A visual mismatch negativity study. Psychology Research, 9(9), 353–370.
Zhao, N., & Zhou, G. (2020). Social media use and mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic: Moderator role of disaster stressor and mediator role of negative affect. Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being, 12(4), 1019–1038.
Zsido, A. N., Deak, A., & Bernath, L. (2019). Is a snake scarier than a gun? The ontogenetic–phylogenetic dispute from a new perspective: The role of arousal. Emotion, 19(4), 726. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000478.
Pisor, A. C. & Jones, J. C. (2021). Human adaptation to climate change: An introduction to the special issue American Journal of Human Biology 33(4) 10.1002/ajhb.v33.4 10.1002/ajhb.23530
Funding
No funds, grants, or other support were received.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
S.S. and L.M. conceived and designed the study and prepared the stimuli. S.S. and O.A. inspected and categorized visual stimuli and analyzed the data. S.S. took the lead in writing the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and reviewed the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethical approval
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic Supplementary Material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Shapouri, S., Martin, L.L. & Arhami, O. Affective Responses to Natural and Technological Disasters; An Evolutionary Perspective. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology 9, 308–322 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-023-00224-z
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-023-00224-z