Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Roles of Mothers’ Partner Satisfaction and Mother-Infant Communication Duration in Mother-Infant Adrenocortical Attunement

  • ORIGINAL ARTICLE
  • Published:
Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Physiological attunement between mothers and infants is emerging as an important indicator of infant information processing, emotion regulation, and health outcomes. Far less understood is the role of paternal support and maternal satisfaction with her partner in facilitating the mother-infant biorhythm. This exploratory study evaluated the impact of maternal satisfaction with her romantic partner and mother-infant communication features on mother-infant hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis attunement. Mothers were asked to complete a series of questionnaires in the presence of her infant, requiring approximately 45 min, occupying maternal attention and serving as a mild challenge for the infant. After completion of the questionnaires, mothers’ and infants’ saliva were collected and later assayed for cortisol. Maternal reports of couple satisfaction (n = 33) impacted mother-infant adrenocortical attunement such that those reporting more satisfaction with their partner showed more attunement with their infants. Maternal reports of couple satisfaction (n = 33) were associated with lower infant, but not lower maternal cortisol levels. Finally, mothers who reported engaging in more infant-directed speech (n = 47), a potential index of maternal sensitivity, showed more attunement with their infants. Removal of an outlier (infant cortisol) reduced the findings to marginal significance in the predicted direction. Though this study is exploratory and the findings are tentative, we conclude that one way that fathers may indirectly contribute to infant outcomes is through social support of the mother, and that infant-directed speech may facilitate mother-infant attunement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Atzil, S., & Barrett, L. F. (2017). Social regulation of allostasis: Commentary on “Mentalizing homeostasis: The social origins of interoceptive inference” by Fotopoulou and Tsakiris. Neuropsychoanalysis, 19, 29–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atzil, S., Touroutoglou, A., Rudy, T., Salcedo, S., Feldman, R., Hooker, J. M., Dickerson, B. C., Catana, C., & Barrett, L. F. (2017). Dopamine in the medial amygdala network mediates human bonding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America (PNAS), 114, 2361–2366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe, B., Alson, D., Jaffe, J., Feldstein, S., & Crown, C. (1988). Vocal congruence in mother-infant play. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 17, 245–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, R. W., Nitschke, W., Gorry, T. H., & Zachman, T. A. (1971). Infantile stimulation and ultrasonic signaling: A possible mediator of early handling phenomena. Developmental Psychobiology, 4, 181–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernard, K., & Dozier, M. (2010). Examining infants' cortisol responses to laboratory tasks among children varying in attachment disorganization: Stress reactivity or return to baseline?. Developmental psychology, 46(6), 1771–1778.

  • Bhaskaran, K., & Smeeth, L. (2014). What is the difference between missing completely at random and missing at random. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43, 1336–1339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 711–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in attachment relationships: Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. I J. Cassidy & P R. Shaver (red.) handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (s. 102-127).

  • Champagne, F., & Meaney, M. J. (2001). Like mother, like daughter: Evidence for non-genomic transmission of parental behavior and stress responsivity. Progress in Brain Research, 133, 287–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coe, C. L., Mendoza, S. P., Smotherman, W. P., & Levine, S. (1978). Mother-infant attachment in the squirrel monkey: Adrenal response to separation. Behavioral Biology, 22, 256–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Mendonça, J. S., Bussab, V. S., Rodrigues, A., Siqueira, J., & Cossette, L. (2012). Postpartum depression, father's involvement, and marital and co-parental relationships from mothers' and fathers' perspectives in a low-income Brazilian sample. Family Science, 3(3–4), 164–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(7), 1562–1592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I., & Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science, 317(5843), 1344–1347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterbrooks, M. A., & Goldberg, W. A. (1984). Toddler development in the family: Impact of father involvement and parenting characteristics. Child Development, 55(3), 740–752.

  • Ellis, B. J., & Boyce, W. T. (2008). Biological sensitivity to context. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 183–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R. (2007). Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. Journal of Child Psychiatry, 48(3), 329–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R., Magori-Cohen, R., Galili, G., Singer, M., & Louzoun, Y. (2011). Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behavior and Development, 34, 569–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Development 60 (6), 1497–1510.

  • Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16(03), 477–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finegood, E. D., Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Hibel, L. C., & Mills-Koonce, R. (2016). Psychobiological influences on maternal sensitivity in the context of adversity. Developmental Psychology, 52(7), 1073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, K. L., Perez, A., Hooper, C., & Claussen, A. H. (2005). Responsiveness and attention during picture-book reading in 18-month-old to 24-month-old toddlers at risk. Early Child Development and Care, 175(1), 63–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flinn, M.V., Quinlan, R.J., Ward, C.V., & Coe, M.K. (2007). Evolution of the human family: Cooperative males, long social childhoods, smart mothers, and extended kin networks. In C. Salmon & T. Shackelford (Eds.), Family relationships (pp. 16-38). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.  

  • Flinn, M. V., Nepomnaschy, P. A., Muehlenbein, M. P., & Ponzi, D. (2011). Evolutionary functions of early social modulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis development in humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1611–1629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fotopoulou, A., & Tsakiris, M. (2017) Mentalizing homeostasis: The social origins of interoceptive inference. Neuropsychoanalysis 19(1), 3–28.

  • Funk, J. L., & Rogge, R. D. (2007). Testing the ruler with item response theory: Increasing precision of measurement for relationship satisfaction with the couples satisfaction index. Journal of Family Psychology, 21(4), 572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C., & Flinn, M. V. (2001). Evolution of human parental behavior and the human family. Parenting, 1(1–2), 5–61.

  • Geary, D. C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Development, 71(1), 57–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghosh, J. K. C., Wilhelm, M. H., Dunkel-Schetter, C., Lombardi, C. A., & Ritz, B. R. (2010). Paternal support and preterm birth, and the moderation of effects of chronic stress: A study in Los Angeles County mothers. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 13(4), 327–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants' babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19(5), 515–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., Can, D. D., Soderstrom, M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2015). (baby)talk to me: The social context of infant-directed speech and its effects on early language acquision. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 339–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordis, E. B., Granger, D. A., Susman, E. J., & Trickett, P. K. (2008). Salivary alpha-amylase-cortisol asymmetry in maltreated youth. Hormones and Behavior, 53, 96–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, I., & Feldman, R. (2008). Synchrony in the triad: A microlevel process model of coparenting and parent-child interactions. Family Processes, 47, 465–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F., & Feldman, R. (2010a). Oxytocin, cortisol, and family triadic interactions. Physiology and Behavior, 101, 679–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F., & Feldman, R. (2010b). Prolactin, oxytocin, and the development of paternal behavior across the first six months of fatherhood. Hormones and Behavior, 58(3), 513–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goslin, M., Booth, A., & Granger, D. A. (2009, April). Baseline cortisol attunement in family dyads: Interpersonal, individual, and contextual moderators. Presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research on child development, Denver, CO.

  • Granger, D. A., Serbin, L. A., Schwartzman, A. E., Lehoux, P., Cooperman, J., & Ikeda, S. (1998). Children’s salivary cortisol, internalizing behavior problems, and family environment: Results from the Concordia longitudinal risk project. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 22, 707–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, T., Striano, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Infants' electric brain responses to emotional prosody. Neuroreport, 16(16), 1825–1828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, T., Oberecker, R., Koch, S. P., & Friederici, A. D. (2010). The developmental origins of voice processing in the human brain. Neuron, 65(6), 852–858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B. (2002). Social regulation of cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27, 199–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hibel, L. C., Granger, D. A., Blair, C., & Cox, M. J. (2009). Intimate partner violence moderates the association between mother–infant adrenocortical activity across an emotional challenge. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(5), 615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hibel, L. C., Trumbell, J. M., & Mercado, E. (2014). Work/non-workday differences in mother, child, and mother–child morning cortisol in a sample of working mothers and their children. Early Human Development, 90(1), 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hibel, L. C., Granger, D. A., Blair, C., & Finegood, E. D. (2015). Maternal-child adrenocortical attunement in early childhood: Continuity and change. Developmental Psychobiology, 57(1), 83–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, M. A. (1987). Early social relationships: A psychobiologist's view. Child Development, 58(3) 633–647.

  • Hrdy, S. B. (2005). Cooperative breeders with an ace in the hole. In E. Voland, A. Chasiotis, & W. Schiefenhövel (Eds.), Grandmotherhood: The evolutionary significance of the second half of female life (pp. 295–317). New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University Press. 

  • Huether, G. (1998). Stress and the adaptive self-organization of neural connectivit during childhood. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 16, 297–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H. R., Vevea, J. L., & Hedges, L. V. (2007). The varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1062.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joosen, K. J., Mesman, J., Backermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Pieper, S., Zeskind, P. S., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2013). Physiological reactivity to infant crying and observed maternal sensitivity. Infancy, 18, 414–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraemer, G. W. (1992). A psychobiological theory of attachment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 493–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krishnakumar, A., & Buehler, C. (2000). Interparental conflict and parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Family Relations, 49(1), 25–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laurent, H. K., Ablow, J. C., & Measelle, J. (2012). Taking stress response out of the box: Stability, discontinuity, and temperament effects on HPA and SNS across social stressors in mother-infant dyads. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 35–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Letourneau, N., Watson, B., Duffett-Leger, L., Hegadoren, K., & Tryphonopoulos, P. (2011). Cortisol patterns of depressed mothers and their infants are related to maternal–infant interactive behaviours. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 29(5), 439–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, S., Rovine, M. J., Cousino Klein, L., & Almeida, D. M. (2013). Synchrony of diurnal cortisol pattern in couples. Journal of Family Psychology, 27(4), 579-588.

  • Maestripieri, D., Hoffman, C. L., Anderson, G. M., Carter, J. D., & Higley, J. D. (2009). Mother-infant interactions in free-ranging macaques: Relationships between physiological and behavioral variables. Physiology & Behavior, 96, 613–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Middlemiss, W., Granger, D. A., Goldberg, W. A., & Nathans, L. (2012). Asynchrony of mother-infant hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity following extinction of infant crying responses induced during the transition to sleep. Early Human Development, 88, 227–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minuchin, P. (1985). Families and individual development: Provocations from the field of family therapy. Child Development, 56(2), 289–302.

  • Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Cooperation and human cognition: The Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 639–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, G. A. (2009). Infants’ and mothers’ vagal reactivity in response to anger. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(11), 1392–1400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Out, D., Pieper, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Zeskind, S., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2010). Intended sensitive and harsh caregiving responses to infant crying: The role of cry pitch and perceived urgency in an adult twin sample. Child Abuse and Neglect, 34, 863–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Out, D., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van Pelt, J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2012). Salivary alpha-amylase and intended harsh caregiving in response to infant crying: Evidence for physiological hyperreactivity. Child Maltreatment, 17, 295–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papp, L. M., Pendry, P., & Adam, E. K. (2009). Mother-adolescent physiological synchrony in naturalistic settings: Within-family cortisol associations and moderators. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(6), 882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raver, C. C. (1996). Relations between social contingency in mother-child interaction and 2-year-olds' social competence. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L. (2011). Recording, transcribing, and coding interaction. In E. Hoff (Ed.), Research Methods in Child Language: A Practical Guide (pp. 191-207). Oxford, U.K.: Wiley-Blackwell. 

  • Ruttle, P. L., Serbin, L. A., Stack, D. M., Schwartzman, A. E., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). Adrenocortical attunement in mother–child dyads: Importance of situational and behavioral characteristics. Biological Psychology, 88(1), 104–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sabbagh, M. A., & Callanan, M. A. (1998). Metarepresentation in action: 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds' developing theories of mind in parent–child conversations. Developmental Psychology, 34(3), 491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxbe, D. E., Adam, E. K., Schetter, C. D., Guardino, C. M., Simon, C., McKinney, C. O., Shalowitz, M. U., & Shriver, E. K. (2015). Cortisol covariation within parents of young children: Moderation by relationship aggression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 121–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sethre-Hofstad, L., Stansbury, K., & Rice, M. A. (2002). Attunement of maternal and child adrenocortical response to child challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27(6), 731–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smotherman, W. P., Wiener, S. G., Mendoza, S. P., & Levine, S. (1977). Responsiveness as a function of differential treatment of rat pups. Psychobiology, 10, 113–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, M. E., & Levine, S. (1985). Brief separation elevates cortisol in mother and infant squirrel monkeys. Physiology & Behavior, 34, 1007–1008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storey, A. E., Noseworthy, D. E., Delahunty, K. M., Halfyard, S. J., & McKay, D. W. (2011). The effects of social context on the hormonal and behavioral responsiveness of human fathers. Hormones and Behavior, 60(4), 353–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. K., & Clark, S. E. (2000). A longitudinal study of mother-child relationships and theory of mind in the preschool period. Social Development, 9(1), 3–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tester-Jones, M., O’Mahen, H., Watkins, E., & Karl, A. (2015). The impact of maternal characteristics, infant temperament and contextual factors on maternal responsiveness to infant. Infant Behavior and Development, 40, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trainor, L. J., Austin, C. M., & Desjardins, R. N. (2000). Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychological Science, 11(3), 188–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uchino, B. N., Uno, D., & Holt-Lunstad, J. (1999). Social support, physiological processes, and health. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(5), 145–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Bakel, H. J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (2008). Adrenocortical and behavioral attunement in parents with 1-year-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 50(2), 196–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Marle, H. J. F., Hermans, E. J., Qin, S., & Fernandez, G. (2009). From specificity to sensitivity: How acute stress affects amygdala processing of biologically salient stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 649–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, J. L., & Levine, S. (1980). Response of mother and infant squirrel monkeys to separation and disturbance. Physiology & Behavior, 24, 829–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, S. F., West, T. V., & Mendes, W. B. (2014). Stress contagion: Physiological covariation between mothers and infants. Psychological Science, 25(4), 934–942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was partially supported by a National Science Foundation-Research Experience for Undergraduates grant (SMA 1358847). The authors would also like to thank Sunny Herron, Lauren Pixley, Stacey Hamilton, Emily Ingram, & Maria Jovanovski for their contribution to the project.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nikki J. Clauss.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors certify that they have NO affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest (such as honoraria; educational grants; participation in speakers’ bureaus; membership, employment, consultancies, stock ownership, or other equity interest; and expert testimony or patent-licensing arrangements), or non-financial interest (such as personal or professional relationships, affiliations, knowledge or beliefs) in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript.

Appendix

Appendix

Mother-Infant Talk Time

Instructions: Please indicate how many minutes you spend talking or singing to your infant during each of the below listed time frames

  1. 1)

    On the typical morning between 6 a.m. and 9 a.m. I spend approximately ______ minutes (out of the 180 min) talking or singing to my infant.

  2. 2)

    On the typical morning between 9 a.m. and noon I spend approximately _______ minutes (out of the 180 min) talking or singing to my infant.

  3. 3)

    On the typical afternoon between noon and 3 p.m. I spend approximately _______ minutes (out of the 180 min) talking or singing to my infant.

  4. 4)

    On the typical afternoon between 3 p.m. and 6 p.m. I spend approximately _______ minutes (out of the 180 min) talking or singing to my infant.

  5. 5)

    On the typical afternoon between 9 p.m. and midnight. I spend approximately _______ minutes (out of the 180 min) talking or singing to my infant.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Clauss, N.J., Byrd-Craven, J., Kennison, S.M. et al. The Roles of Mothers’ Partner Satisfaction and Mother-Infant Communication Duration in Mother-Infant Adrenocortical Attunement. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology 4, 91–107 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-017-0078-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-017-0078-8

Keywords

Navigation