Skip to main content
Log in

Facial Masculinity and Beardedness Determine Men’s Explicit, but Not Their Implicit, Responses to Male Dominance

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Sexual dimorphism in facial shape and beardedness are salient human secondary sexual traits that enhance perceptions of men’s social dominance. The majority of this evidence, however, comes from studies measuring explicit ratings. To our knowledge, few studies have tested whether facial masculinity and beardedness are implicitly associated with dominance. In the current study, we use a within-subjects design to test whether facial masculinity and beardedness drive implicit reactions and overt ratings of male dominance. Participants viewed stimuli depicting the same men when clean-shaven, with heavy stubble, and fully bearded that were morphed to be either more masculine or less masculine using computer graphic software. Participants completed an affective priming word categorisation task as well as explicit ratings of social dominance. No facilitation effects were observed for masculinised or bearded faces on implicit judgements relating to dominance. In contrast, results revealed that masculinized versions of clean-shaven, stubbled and fully bearded faces received higher explicit dominance ratings than feminized versions. However, the effects of facial masculinity were largest within clean-shaven stimuli and decreased as faces became more hirsute, suggesting that facial masculinity had diminishing returns on dominance ratings. Our results support a role for masculine facial shape and beardedness in explicit, but not implicit, judgments of dominance among men.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(3–4), 249–266. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banse, R. (2001). Affective priming with liked and disliked persons: prime visibility determines congruency and incongruency effects. Cognition and Emotion, 15(4), 501–520. doi:10.1080/0269993004200213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barber, N. (2001). Mustache fashion covaries with a good marriage market for women. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25(4), 261–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cogntion (Vol. 10., Vol. Book, Whole). Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

  • Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Facial expressions, their communicatory functions and neuro–cognitive substrates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 561–572. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R. C., Shelly, J. P., Jordan, L. A., & Dixson, B. J. W. (2015). The multivariate evolution of female body shape in an artificial digital ecosystem. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(5), 351–358. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L. G., Perrett, D. I., Penton-Voak, I. S., et al. (2006). Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner’s masculinity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1592), 1355–1360. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., & Brooks, R. C. (2013). The role of facial hair in women’s perceptions of men’s attractiveness, health, masculinity and parenting abilities. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 236–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J. W., & Rantala, M. (2016). The role of facial and body hair distribution in Women’s judgments of Men’s sexual attractiveness. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 877–889.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., & Vasey, P. L. (2012). Beards augment perceptions of men’s age, social status, and aggressiveness, but not attractiveness. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 481–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, A. F., Dixson, B. J., & Anderson, M. (2005). Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Visually Conspicuous Sexually Dimorphic Traits in Male Monkeys, Apes, and Human Beings. Annual Review of Sex Research, 16(1053–2528), 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., Vasey, P., Sagata, K., Sibanda, N., Linklater, W., & Dixson, A. (2011). Men’s preferences for Women’s breast morphology in New Zealand, Samoa, and Papua New Guinea. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40(6), 1271–1279. doi:10.1007/s10508-010-9680-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., Tam, J. C., & Awasthy, M. (2013). Do women’s preferences for men’s facial hair change with reproductive status? Behavioral Ecology, 24(3), 708–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., Grimshaw, G. M., Ormsby, D. K., & Dixson, A. F. (2014). Eye-tracking women’s preferences for men’s somatotypes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(2), 73–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixson, B. J., Duncan, M., & Dixson, A. (2015). The role of breast size and areolar pigmentation in perceptions of Women’s sexual attractiveness, reproductive health, sexual maturity, maternal nurturing abilities, and age. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(6), 1685–1695. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0516-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emlen, D. J. (2008). The evolution of animal weapons. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 39(1), 387–413. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.39.110707.173502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1508–1519. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: an overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15(2), 115–141. doi:10.1080/0269993004200024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229–238. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1013–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feinman, S., & Gill, G. W. (1977). Females’ response to males’ beardedness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44(2), 533–534. doi:10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fink, B., Neave, N., & Seydel, H. (2007). Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. American Journal of Human Biology, 19(1), 82–87. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, D. A., & Haselton, M. G. (2007). Why is muscularity sexy? Tests of the fitness indicator hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(8), 1167–1183. doi:10.1177/0146167207303022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gangestad, S. W., & Scheyd, G. J. (2005). The evolution of human physical attractiveness. Annual Review of Anthropology, 34(1), 523–548. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.33.070203.143733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geniole, S. N., & McCormick, C. M. (2015). Facing our ancestors: judgements of aggression are consistent and related to the facial width-to-height ratio in men irrespective of beards. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 279–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geniole, S. N., Denson, T. F., Dixson, B. J., Carré, J. M., & McCormick, C. M. (2015). Evidence from meta-analyses of the facial width-to-height ratio as an evolved cue of threat. PloS One, 10(7), e0132726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. R. (2014). Do women’s mate preferences change across the ovulatory cycle? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140(5), 1205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grammer, K., Fink, B., MØLler, A. P., & Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics: sexual selection and the biology of beauty. Biological Reviews, 78(3), 385–407. doi:10.1017/s1464793102006085.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grueter, C. C., Isler, K., & Dixson, B. J. (2015). Are badges of status adaptive in large complex primate groups? Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(5), 398–406. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.03.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, R. G., & Brothwell, D. R. (1969). Biosocial aspects of Ainu hirsuteness. Journal of Biosocial Science, 1(02), 109–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holzleitner, I. J., & Perrett, D. I. (2015). Perception of strength from 3D faces is linked to facial cues of physique. Evolution and Human Behavior.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hundhammer, T., & Mussweiler, T. (2012). How sex puts you in gendered shoes: sexuality-priming leads to gender-based self-perception and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(1), 176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janif, Z. J., Brooks, R. C., & Dixson, B. J. (2014). Negative frequency-dependent preferences and variation in male facial hair. Biology Letters, 10(4), 20130958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, B. G. (2016). DirectRT. (2010.2.103.1115 ed.). New York, NY: Empirisoft Corporation

  • Klauer, K. C. (1997). Affective priming. European Review of Social Psychology, 8(1), 67–103. doi:10.1080/14792779643000083.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K. C, & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: findings and theories. In J. Musch, & K. C. Klaur (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Knussman, R., & Christiansen, K. (1988). Attributes of masculinity and androgen level. Homo, 39, 45–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kokko, H., Brooks, R., Jennions, M. D., & Morley, J. (2003). The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 270(1515), 653–664. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koranyi, N., Gast, A., & Rothermund, K. (2013). “although quite nice, I was somehow not attracted by that person”: attitudes toward romantically committed opposite-sex others are immune to positive evaluative conditioning. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(4), 403–410. doi:10.1177/1948550612467037.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Law Smith, M. J., Perrett, D. I., Jones, B. C., Cornwell, R. E., Moore, F. R., Feinberg, D. R., et al. (2006). Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1583), 135–140. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, T. T., & Lu, Y. (2014). The subliminal affective priming effects of faces displaying various levels of arousal: an ERP study. Neuorscience Letters, 583, 148–153. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2014.09.027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 1638–1659. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Livingston, R. W., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). What are we really priming? Cue-based versus category-based processing of facial stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 5–18. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maddox, K. B. (2004). Perspectives on racial phenotypicality bias. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 383–401. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maddox, K. B., & Dukes, K. N. (2008). Social categorization and beyod: how facial features impact social judgements. In N. Ambady & J. J. Skowronski (Eds.), First impressions (pp. 205–233). New York: NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcinkowska, U. M., Kozlov, M. V., Cai, H., Contreras-Garduño, J., Dixson, B. J., Oana, G. A., et al. (2014). Cross-cultural variation in men’s preference for sexual dimorphism in women’s faces. Biology Letters, 10(4), 20130850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muscarella, F., & Cunningham, M. R. (1996). The evolutionary significance and social perception of male pattern baldness and facial hair. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17(2), 99–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neave, N., & Shields, K. (2008). The effects of facial hair manipulation on female perceptions of attractiveness, masculinity, and dominance in male faces. [doi:10.1016/j.Paid.2008.05.007]. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(5), 373–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oldmeadow, J. A., & Dixson, B. J. (2016). The association between Men’s sexist attitudes and facial hair. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 891–899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palermo, R., & Schmalzl, L. (2006). Affective priming from faces: the influence of spatial frequency, prime duration and amygdala damage. Australian Journal of Psychology, 58, 87–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, R. J. (1973). Impressions of the male personality as a function of beardedness. Psychology, 10(1), 29–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. [10.1038/29772]. Nature, 394(6696), 884–887.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pound, N., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Surridge, A. K. (2009). Testosterone responses to competition in men are related to facial masculinity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1654), 153–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. [doi: 10.1016/j.Evolhumbehav.2010.02.005]. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(3), 157–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. (2016). Human sexual selection. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 28–32. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. [doi: 10.1016/j.Evolhumbehav.2005.11.003]. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 283–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A., Bailey, D. H., & Reno, P. L. (2015). Contest competition in men. In The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology: John Wiley & Sons, Inc..

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, J. A., & Blunk, E. M. (1990). The influence of facial hair on impression formation. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 18(1), 169–175. doi:10.2224/sbp.1990.18.1.169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. In Annual review of psychology (Vol. 57, pp. 199–226, Annual Review of Psychology). Palo Alto: Annual Reviews

  • Roberts, S. C., Owen, R. C., & Havlicek, J. (2010). Distinguishing between perceiver and wearer effects in clothing color-associated attributions. Evolutionary Psychology, 8(3), 350–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez, D. T., Kiefer, A. K., & Ybarra, O. (2006). Sexual submissiveness in women: costs for sexual autonomy and arousal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(4), 512–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxton, T. K., Mackey, L. L., McCarty, K., & Neave, N. (2016). A lover or a fighter? Opposing sexual selection pressures on men’s vocal pitch and facial hair. Behavioral Ecology, 27(2), 512–519. doi:10.1093/beheco/arv178.

  • Scott, I., Clark, A., Boothroyd, L., & Penton-Voak, I. (2013). Do men’s faces really signal heritable immunocompetence? Behavioral Ecology, 24, 579–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, I. M., Clark, A. P., Josephson, S. C., Boyette, A. H., Cuthill, I. C., Fried, R. L., et al. (2014). Human preferences for sexually dimorphic faces may be evolutionarily novel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(40), 14388–14393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., & Gurven, M. (2009). Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276(1656), 575–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sell, A., Hone, L. E., & Pound, N. (2012). The importance of physical strength to human males. Human Nature, 23(1), 30–44. doi:10.1007/s12110-012-9131-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheehan, M. J., & Bergman, T. J. (2016). A quality signaling–recognition trade-off at the level of the type of interaction not species: a response to comments on Sheehan and Bergman. Behavioral Ecology, 27, 2–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singh, D., Dixson, B. J., Jessop, T. S., Morgan, B., & Dixson, A. F. (2010). Cross-cultural consensus for waist–hip ratio and women’s attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(3), 176–181. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skowronski, J. J., Sedikides, C., Heider, J. D., Wood, S. E., & Scherer, C. R. (2010). On the road to self-perception: interpretation of self-behaviors can Be altered by priming. Journal of Personality, 78(1), 361–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spisak, B. R., Dekker, P. H., Krüger, M., & Van Vugt, M. (2012). Warriors and peacekeepers: testing a biosocial implicit leadership hypothesis of intergroup relations using masculine and feminine faces. PloS One, 7(1), e30399–e30399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terburg, D., Hooiveld, N., Aarts, H., Kenemans, J. L., & van Honk, J. (2011). Eye tracking unconscious face-to-face Confrontations: Dominance Motives Prolong Gaze to Masked Angry Faces. Psychological Science, doi:10.1177/0956797611398492.

  • Tiddeman, B. P., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Computer graphics in facial perception research. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 21(5), 42–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Berghe, P. L., & Frost, P. (1986). Skin color preference, sexual dimorphism and sexual selection: a case of gene culture co-evolution?. [doi: 10.1080/01419870.1986.9993516]. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 9(1), 87–113. doi:10.1080/01419870.1986.9993516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, C. D., Fraccaro, P. J., Smith, F. G., Vukovic, J., Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C. (2010a). Taller men are less sensitive to cues of dominance in other men. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 943–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, C. D., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2010b). Individual differences in dominance perception: dominant men are less sensitive to facial cues of male dominance. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 967–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welling, L. L., Moreau, B. J., Bird, B. M., Hansen, S., & Carré, J. M. (2016). Exogenous testosterone increases men’s perceptions of their own physical dominance. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 64, 136–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, J. C. (2007). Sexual dimorphism of body composition. [doi: 10.1016/j.Beem.2007.04.007]. Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 21(3), 415–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J. O., Gilani, S. Z., Shafait, F., Mian, A., Tan, D. W., Maybery, M. T., et al. (2015). Prenatal testosterone exposure is related to sexually dimorphic facial morphology in adulthood. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282(1816). doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.1351.

  • Windhager, S., Schaefer, K., & Fink, B. (2011). Geometric morphometrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength and perceived attractiveness, dominance, and masculinity. American Journal of Human Biology, 23(6), 805–814. doi:10.1002/ajhb.21219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkler, E. M., & Christiansen, K. (1993). Sex hormone levels and body hair growth in! Kung san and Kavango men from Namibia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 92(2), 155–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wogalter, M. S., & Hosie, J. A. (1991). Effects of cranial and facial hair on perceptions of age and person. [article]. Journal of Social Psychology, 131(4), 589–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D. R. (1986). Self-perceived masculinity between bearded and nonbearded males. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62(3), 769–770. doi:10.2466/pms.1986.62.3.769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, J., Cao, Z., Xu, X., & Chen, G. (2012). The amygdala is involved in affective priming effect for fearful faces. Brain and Cognition, 80(1), 15–22. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2012.04.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, S., Taylor, A., Rubino, C., & Barton, J. (2015). Valence, expression and identity effects in the affective priming paradigm. Journal of Vision, 15(12), 1375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zietsch, B. P., Lee, A. J., Sherlock, J. M., & Jern, P. (2015). Variation in Women’s preferences regarding male facial masculinity is better explained by genetic differences than by previously identified context-dependent effects. Psychological Science, 26, 1440–1448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Barnaby J. W. Dixson.

Ethics declarations

Ethics Statement

All protocols were approved by the Human Ethics Committee at the University of Queensland and are in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided informed consent before participating in this study and were free to withdraw from the research at any point.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sherlock, J.M., Tegg, B., Sulikowski, D. et al. Facial Masculinity and Beardedness Determine Men’s Explicit, but Not Their Implicit, Responses to Male Dominance. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology 3, 14–29 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-016-0047-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-016-0047-7

Keywords

Navigation