Abstract
Sexual dimorphism in facial shape and beardedness are salient human secondary sexual traits that enhance perceptions of men’s social dominance. The majority of this evidence, however, comes from studies measuring explicit ratings. To our knowledge, few studies have tested whether facial masculinity and beardedness are implicitly associated with dominance. In the current study, we use a within-subjects design to test whether facial masculinity and beardedness drive implicit reactions and overt ratings of male dominance. Participants viewed stimuli depicting the same men when clean-shaven, with heavy stubble, and fully bearded that were morphed to be either more masculine or less masculine using computer graphic software. Participants completed an affective priming word categorisation task as well as explicit ratings of social dominance. No facilitation effects were observed for masculinised or bearded faces on implicit judgements relating to dominance. In contrast, results revealed that masculinized versions of clean-shaven, stubbled and fully bearded faces received higher explicit dominance ratings than feminized versions. However, the effects of facial masculinity were largest within clean-shaven stimuli and decreased as faces became more hirsute, suggesting that facial masculinity had diminishing returns on dominance ratings. Our results support a role for masculine facial shape and beardedness in explicit, but not implicit, judgments of dominance among men.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(3–4), 249–266. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990951.
Banse, R. (2001). Affective priming with liked and disliked persons: prime visibility determines congruency and incongruency effects. Cognition and Emotion, 15(4), 501–520. doi:10.1080/0269993004200213.
Barber, N. (2001). Mustache fashion covaries with a good marriage market for women. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25(4), 261–272.
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cogntion (Vol. 10., Vol. Book, Whole). Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Facial expressions, their communicatory functions and neuro–cognitive substrates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 561–572. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1220.
Brooks, R. C., Shelly, J. P., Jordan, L. A., & Dixson, B. J. W. (2015). The multivariate evolution of female body shape in an artificial digital ecosystem. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(5), 351–358. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.02.001.
DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L. G., Perrett, D. I., Penton-Voak, I. S., et al. (2006). Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner’s masculinity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1592), 1355–1360. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3445.
Dixson, B. J., & Brooks, R. C. (2013). The role of facial hair in women’s perceptions of men’s attractiveness, health, masculinity and parenting abilities. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 236–241.
Dixson, B. J. W., & Rantala, M. (2016). The role of facial and body hair distribution in Women’s judgments of Men’s sexual attractiveness. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 877–889.
Dixson, B. J., & Vasey, P. L. (2012). Beards augment perceptions of men’s age, social status, and aggressiveness, but not attractiveness. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 481–490.
Dixson, A. F., Dixson, B. J., & Anderson, M. (2005). Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Visually Conspicuous Sexually Dimorphic Traits in Male Monkeys, Apes, and Human Beings. Annual Review of Sex Research, 16(1053–2528), 1–19.
Dixson, B. J., Vasey, P., Sagata, K., Sibanda, N., Linklater, W., & Dixson, A. (2011). Men’s preferences for Women’s breast morphology in New Zealand, Samoa, and Papua New Guinea. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40(6), 1271–1279. doi:10.1007/s10508-010-9680-6.
Dixson, B. J., Tam, J. C., & Awasthy, M. (2013). Do women’s preferences for men’s facial hair change with reproductive status? Behavioral Ecology, 24(3), 708–716.
Dixson, B. J., Grimshaw, G. M., Ormsby, D. K., & Dixson, A. F. (2014). Eye-tracking women’s preferences for men’s somatotypes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(2), 73–79.
Dixson, B. J., Duncan, M., & Dixson, A. (2015). The role of breast size and areolar pigmentation in perceptions of Women’s sexual attractiveness, reproductive health, sexual maturity, maternal nurturing abilities, and age. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(6), 1685–1695. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0516-2.
Emlen, D. J. (2008). The evolution of animal weapons. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 39(1), 387–413. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.39.110707.173502.
Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1508–1519. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508.
Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: an overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15(2), 115–141. doi:10.1080/0269993004200024.
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229–238. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229.
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: a bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1013–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013.
Feinman, S., & Gill, G. W. (1977). Females’ response to males’ beardedness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44(2), 533–534. doi:10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.533.
Fink, B., Neave, N., & Seydel, H. (2007). Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. American Journal of Human Biology, 19(1), 82–87. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20583.
Frederick, D. A., & Haselton, M. G. (2007). Why is muscularity sexy? Tests of the fitness indicator hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(8), 1167–1183. doi:10.1177/0146167207303022.
Gangestad, S. W., & Scheyd, G. J. (2005). The evolution of human physical attractiveness. Annual Review of Anthropology, 34(1), 523–548. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.33.070203.143733.
Geniole, S. N., & McCormick, C. M. (2015). Facing our ancestors: judgements of aggression are consistent and related to the facial width-to-height ratio in men irrespective of beards. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 279–285.
Geniole, S. N., Denson, T. F., Dixson, B. J., Carré, J. M., & McCormick, C. M. (2015). Evidence from meta-analyses of the facial width-to-height ratio as an evolved cue of threat. PloS One, 10(7), e0132726.
Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. R. (2014). Do women’s mate preferences change across the ovulatory cycle? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140(5), 1205.
Grammer, K., Fink, B., MØLler, A. P., & Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics: sexual selection and the biology of beauty. Biological Reviews, 78(3), 385–407. doi:10.1017/s1464793102006085.
Grueter, C. C., Isler, K., & Dixson, B. J. (2015). Are badges of status adaptive in large complex primate groups? Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(5), 398–406. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.03.003.
Harvey, R. G., & Brothwell, D. R. (1969). Biosocial aspects of Ainu hirsuteness. Journal of Biosocial Science, 1(02), 109–124.
Holzleitner, I. J., & Perrett, D. I. (2015). Perception of strength from 3D faces is linked to facial cues of physique. Evolution and Human Behavior.
Hundhammer, T., & Mussweiler, T. (2012). How sex puts you in gendered shoes: sexuality-priming leads to gender-based self-perception and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(1), 176.
Janif, Z. J., Brooks, R. C., & Dixson, B. J. (2014). Negative frequency-dependent preferences and variation in male facial hair. Biology Letters, 10(4), 20130958.
Jarvis, B. G. (2016). DirectRT. (2010.2.103.1115 ed.). New York, NY: Empirisoft Corporation
Klauer, K. C. (1997). Affective priming. European Review of Social Psychology, 8(1), 67–103. doi:10.1080/14792779643000083.
Klauer, K. C, & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: findings and theories. In J. Musch, & K. C. Klaur (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Knussman, R., & Christiansen, K. (1988). Attributes of masculinity and androgen level. Homo, 39, 45–50.
Kokko, H., Brooks, R., Jennions, M. D., & Morley, J. (2003). The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 270(1515), 653–664. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2235.
Koranyi, N., Gast, A., & Rothermund, K. (2013). “although quite nice, I was somehow not attracted by that person”: attitudes toward romantically committed opposite-sex others are immune to positive evaluative conditioning. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(4), 403–410. doi:10.1177/1948550612467037.
Law Smith, M. J., Perrett, D. I., Jones, B. C., Cornwell, R. E., Moore, F. R., Feinberg, D. R., et al. (2006). Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1583), 135–140. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3296.
Li, T. T., & Lu, Y. (2014). The subliminal affective priming effects of faces displaying various levels of arousal: an ERP study. Neuorscience Letters, 583, 148–153. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2014.09.027.
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 1638–1659. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0404.
Livingston, R. W., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). What are we really priming? Cue-based versus category-based processing of facial stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 5–18. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.5.
Maddox, K. B. (2004). Perspectives on racial phenotypicality bias. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 383–401. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_4.
Maddox, K. B., & Dukes, K. N. (2008). Social categorization and beyod: how facial features impact social judgements. In N. Ambady & J. J. Skowronski (Eds.), First impressions (pp. 205–233). New York: NY: Guilford.
Marcinkowska, U. M., Kozlov, M. V., Cai, H., Contreras-Garduño, J., Dixson, B. J., Oana, G. A., et al. (2014). Cross-cultural variation in men’s preference for sexual dimorphism in women’s faces. Biology Letters, 10(4), 20130850.
Muscarella, F., & Cunningham, M. R. (1996). The evolutionary significance and social perception of male pattern baldness and facial hair. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17(2), 99–117.
Neave, N., & Shields, K. (2008). The effects of facial hair manipulation on female perceptions of attractiveness, masculinity, and dominance in male faces. [doi:10.1016/j.Paid.2008.05.007]. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(5), 373–377.
Oldmeadow, J. A., & Dixson, B. J. (2016). The association between Men’s sexist attitudes and facial hair. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 891–899.
Palermo, R., & Schmalzl, L. (2006). Affective priming from faces: the influence of spatial frequency, prime duration and amygdala damage. Australian Journal of Psychology, 58, 87–87.
Pellegrini, R. J. (1973). Impressions of the male personality as a function of beardedness. Psychology, 10(1), 29–33.
Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. [10.1038/29772]. Nature, 394(6696), 884–887.
Pound, N., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Surridge, A. K. (2009). Testosterone responses to competition in men are related to facial masculinity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1654), 153–159.
Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. [doi: 10.1016/j.Evolhumbehav.2010.02.005]. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(3), 157–175.
Puts, D. (2016). Human sexual selection. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 28–32. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.011.
Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. [doi: 10.1016/j.Evolhumbehav.2005.11.003]. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 283–296.
Puts, D. A., Bailey, D. H., & Reno, P. L. (2015). Contest competition in men. In The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology: John Wiley & Sons, Inc..
Reed, J. A., & Blunk, E. M. (1990). The influence of facial hair on impression formation. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 18(1), 169–175. doi:10.2224/sbp.1990.18.1.169.
Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. In Annual review of psychology (Vol. 57, pp. 199–226, Annual Review of Psychology). Palo Alto: Annual Reviews
Roberts, S. C., Owen, R. C., & Havlicek, J. (2010). Distinguishing between perceiver and wearer effects in clothing color-associated attributions. Evolutionary Psychology, 8(3), 350–364.
Sanchez, D. T., Kiefer, A. K., & Ybarra, O. (2006). Sexual submissiveness in women: costs for sexual autonomy and arousal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(4), 512–524.
Saxton, T. K., Mackey, L. L., McCarty, K., & Neave, N. (2016). A lover or a fighter? Opposing sexual selection pressures on men’s vocal pitch and facial hair. Behavioral Ecology, 27(2), 512–519. doi:10.1093/beheco/arv178.
Scott, I., Clark, A., Boothroyd, L., & Penton-Voak, I. (2013). Do men’s faces really signal heritable immunocompetence? Behavioral Ecology, 24, 579–589.
Scott, I. M., Clark, A. P., Josephson, S. C., Boyette, A. H., Cuthill, I. C., Fried, R. L., et al. (2014). Human preferences for sexually dimorphic faces may be evolutionarily novel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(40), 14388–14393.
Sell, A., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., & Gurven, M. (2009). Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276(1656), 575–584.
Sell, A., Hone, L. E., & Pound, N. (2012). The importance of physical strength to human males. Human Nature, 23(1), 30–44. doi:10.1007/s12110-012-9131-2.
Sheehan, M. J., & Bergman, T. J. (2016). A quality signaling–recognition trade-off at the level of the type of interaction not species: a response to comments on Sheehan and Bergman. Behavioral Ecology, 27, 2–13.
Singh, D., Dixson, B. J., Jessop, T. S., Morgan, B., & Dixson, A. F. (2010). Cross-cultural consensus for waist–hip ratio and women’s attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(3), 176–181. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.09.001.
Skowronski, J. J., Sedikides, C., Heider, J. D., Wood, S. E., & Scherer, C. R. (2010). On the road to self-perception: interpretation of self-behaviors can Be altered by priming. Journal of Personality, 78(1), 361–391.
Spisak, B. R., Dekker, P. H., Krüger, M., & Van Vugt, M. (2012). Warriors and peacekeepers: testing a biosocial implicit leadership hypothesis of intergroup relations using masculine and feminine faces. PloS One, 7(1), e30399–e30399.
Terburg, D., Hooiveld, N., Aarts, H., Kenemans, J. L., & van Honk, J. (2011). Eye tracking unconscious face-to-face Confrontations: Dominance Motives Prolong Gaze to Masked Angry Faces. Psychological Science, doi:10.1177/0956797611398492.
Tiddeman, B. P., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Computer graphics in facial perception research. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 21(5), 42–50.
van den Berghe, P. L., & Frost, P. (1986). Skin color preference, sexual dimorphism and sexual selection: a case of gene culture co-evolution?. [doi: 10.1080/01419870.1986.9993516]. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 9(1), 87–113. doi:10.1080/01419870.1986.9993516.
Watkins, C. D., Fraccaro, P. J., Smith, F. G., Vukovic, J., Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C. (2010a). Taller men are less sensitive to cues of dominance in other men. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 943–947.
Watkins, C. D., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2010b). Individual differences in dominance perception: dominant men are less sensitive to facial cues of male dominance. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 967–971.
Welling, L. L., Moreau, B. J., Bird, B. M., Hansen, S., & Carré, J. M. (2016). Exogenous testosterone increases men’s perceptions of their own physical dominance. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 64, 136–142.
Wells, J. C. (2007). Sexual dimorphism of body composition. [doi: 10.1016/j.Beem.2007.04.007]. Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 21(3), 415–430.
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Gilani, S. Z., Shafait, F., Mian, A., Tan, D. W., Maybery, M. T., et al. (2015). Prenatal testosterone exposure is related to sexually dimorphic facial morphology in adulthood. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282(1816). doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.1351.
Windhager, S., Schaefer, K., & Fink, B. (2011). Geometric morphometrics of male facial shape in relation to physical strength and perceived attractiveness, dominance, and masculinity. American Journal of Human Biology, 23(6), 805–814. doi:10.1002/ajhb.21219.
Winkler, E. M., & Christiansen, K. (1993). Sex hormone levels and body hair growth in! Kung san and Kavango men from Namibia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 92(2), 155–164.
Wogalter, M. S., & Hosie, J. A. (1991). Effects of cranial and facial hair on perceptions of age and person. [article]. Journal of Social Psychology, 131(4), 589–591.
Wood, D. R. (1986). Self-perceived masculinity between bearded and nonbearded males. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62(3), 769–770. doi:10.2466/pms.1986.62.3.769.
Yang, J., Cao, Z., Xu, X., & Chen, G. (2012). The amygdala is involved in affective priming effect for fearful faces. Brain and Cognition, 80(1), 15–22. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2012.04.005.
Yeung, S., Taylor, A., Rubino, C., & Barton, J. (2015). Valence, expression and identity effects in the affective priming paradigm. Journal of Vision, 15(12), 1375.
Zietsch, B. P., Lee, A. J., Sherlock, J. M., & Jern, P. (2015). Variation in Women’s preferences regarding male facial masculinity is better explained by genetic differences than by previously identified context-dependent effects. Psychological Science, 26, 1440–1448.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics Statement
All protocols were approved by the Human Ethics Committee at the University of Queensland and are in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided informed consent before participating in this study and were free to withdraw from the research at any point.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Sherlock, J.M., Tegg, B., Sulikowski, D. et al. Facial Masculinity and Beardedness Determine Men’s Explicit, but Not Their Implicit, Responses to Male Dominance. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology 3, 14–29 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-016-0047-7
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-016-0047-7