The Psychological Record

, Volume 66, Issue 1, pp 191–199 | Cite as

Priority Disputes in the History of Psychology with Special Attention to the Franz–Kalischer Dispute About Who First Combined Animal Training with Brain Extirpation to Investigate Brain Functions

Historical Article

Abstract

Shepherd Ivory Franz (American) and Otto Kalischer (German) each claimed to have been the first to combine animal training and brain extirpation to study brain function, a methodological approach that historians assert fundamentally changed subsequent neuropsychological research. Each defended his claim in 1907 in back-to-back commentaries in the journal Zentralblatt für Physiologie. Before considering details of the Franz versus Kalischer dispute, it was deemed useful to consider priority disputes in general and to revisit the priority claims for who discovered the “conditioned reflex” and whether Pierre Flourens was the “father” of brain extirpation as examples of this type of research. Consideration of the Franz–Kalischer dispute began with a brief history of the study of brain function to provide background and context for the Franz–Kalischer dispute. For additional context, biographic sketches of Franz and Kalischer are presented. Then, details of the dispute are presented and discussed followed by conclusions that include that Franz (The American Journal of Physiology, 8, 1–22, 1902) preceded Kalischer (1907a) and that it is highly unlikely that anyone before Franz had used his combination of innovative methods. Finally, the perceived importance of being first to combine animal training with brain extirpation is represented by quotations from several authors of history or psychology textbooks and one author of a history of neuroscience textbook.

Keywords

Animal training Animal learning Brain ablation Brain extirpation Brain lesion History brain research Learning Memory Priority in psychology Priority in science 

References

Section A: Recent (1991–2012) History of Psychology Textbooks Examined

  1. Benjafield, J. G. (2005). A history of psychology (2nd ed.). Ontario, Canada: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Benjamin, L. T., Jr. (1997). A history of psychology: Original sources and contemporary research. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  3. Benjamin, L. T., Jr. (2007). A history of modern psychology. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  4. Bolles, R. C. (1993). The story of psychology: A thematic history. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
  5. Brennan, J. F. (2003). History and systems of psychology (6th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  6. Fancher, R. E. (1996). Pioneers of psychology (3rd ed.). New York, NY: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  7. Fancher, R. E., & Rutherford, A. (2012). Pioneers of psychology (4th ed.). New York, NY: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  8. Goodwin, C. J. (2008). A history of modern psychology (3rd ed.). New York. NY: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  9. Hergenhahn, B. R. (2009). An introduction to the history of psychology (6th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth.Google Scholar
  10. Hothersall, D. (2004). History of psychology (4th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  11. Hunt, M. (1993). The story of psychology. New York, NY: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  12. Krapp, K. (Ed.). (2005). Psychologists and their theories for students. Volume 1: A-K, Volume 2: L.-Z. Detroit, MI: Thomson-Gale.Google Scholar
  13. Leahey, T. H. (2001). A history of modern psychology (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  14. Leahey, T. H. (2004). A history of psychology: Main currents in psychological thought (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  15. O’Boyle, C. G. (2006). History of psychology: A cultural perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  16. Schultz, D. P., & Schultz, S. E. (2004). A history of modern psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth.Google Scholar
  17. Smith, R. (1997). The Norton history of the human sciences. London, England: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  18. Thorne, B. M., & Henley, T. B. (2005). Connections in the history and systems of psychology (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  19. Viney, W., & King, D. B. (2003). A history of psychology: Ideas and context (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  20. Watson, R. I., & Evans, R. B. (1991). The great psychologists: A history of psychological thought (5th ed.). New York, NY: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
  21. Wertheimer, M. (2000). A brief history of psychology (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth.Google Scholar

Section B: General References

  1. Boring, E. G. (1929). A history of experimental psychology. New York, NY: D. Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  2. Boring, E. G. (1950). A history of experimental psychology (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  3. Brannigan, A. (1981). The social basis of scientific discoveries. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Brodal, A. (1981). Neurological anatomy in relation to clinical medicine (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Colotla, V. A., & Bach-y-Rita, P. (2002). Shepherd Ivory Franz: His contributions to neuropsychology and rehabilitation. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 141–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Coon, D. J. (1982). Eponymy, obscurity, Twitmyer, and Pavlov. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 18, 255–262.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Dallenbach, K. M. (1959). Twitmyer and the conditioned reflex. American Journal of Psychology, 72, 633–638.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Diamond, I. T. (1979). The subdivisions of neocortex: A proposal to revise the traditional view of sensory, motor, and association areas. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, 8, 1–43.Google Scholar
  9. Finger, S. (1994). Origins of neuroscience: A history of explorations into brain function. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Franz, S. I. (1902). On the functions of the cerebrum: I. The frontal lobes in relation to the production and retention of simple sensory-motor habits. The American Journal of Physiology, 8, 1–22.Google Scholar
  11. Franz, S. I. (1906). Observations of the functions of the association areas (cerebrum) in monkeys. Journal of the American Medical Association, 47, 1464–1467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Franz, S. I. (1907a). Über die sogenannte Dressurmethode für Zentralnervensystems-untersuchungen. [About the so-called training method for the study of the central nervous system.] Zentralblatt für Physiologie, XXL, 583–584.Google Scholar
  13. Franz, S. I. (1907b). On the functions of the cerebrum: The frontal lobes. In R. S. Woodworth (Ed.), Archives of psychology (pp. 1–64). New York, NY: The Science Press.Google Scholar
  14. Franz, S. I. (1912a). New phrenology. Science, 35, 321–328.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Franz, S. I. (1912b). Handbook of mental examination methods. New York, NY: The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing.Google Scholar
  16. Franz, S. I. (1932). Shepherd Ivory Franz. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 2, pp. 89–113). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Furedy, J. J. (2004). Pavlovian George Windholz (1931–2002): An exemplar of scholarly “observation and observation” and a critical contributor to psychology, and hence to behavioral neuroscience. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 39, 139–147.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Gantt, W. H. (1928a). Lectures on conditioned reflexes (Vol. 1: Ivan Petrovitch Pavlov). New York, NY: International Publishers.Google Scholar
  19. Gantt, W. H. (1928b). Lectures on conditioned reflexes (Vol. 2, Conditioned reflexes and psychiatry, Ivan Petrovitch Pavlov). New York, NY: International Publishers.Google Scholar
  20. Hamilton, A., & Jackson, J. B. (1969). UCLA on the move, during fifty golden years, 1919–1969. Los Angeles, CA: Ward Ritchie Press.Google Scholar
  21. Kalischer, O. (1907a). Zur Funktion des Schläfenluppens des Grosshims. Eine nue Hörprüfungsmethod bei Hunden; zugleich ein Beitrag zur Dressur als physiologischer Untersuchungsmethode. [Toward the function of the temporal lobes of the cerebral cortex. A new auditory-test-method in dogs as well as a contribution of training as a physiological method.] Sitzungsberichte der phys-math, Klasse, Bericte der Königlichen reussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften, Berlin, 204–217. [This reference was copied verbatim from Windholz and Lamal, 1993, p. 349.]Google Scholar
  22. Kalischer, O. (1907b). Einige bemerkungen über meine dressurmethode. [Some comments about my training method.] Zentralblatt für Physiologie, XXL, 585–586.Google Scholar
  23. Krech, D. (1964). Cortical localization of function. In L. Postman (Ed.), Psychology in the making: Histories of selected research problems (pp. 31–72). New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
  24. Kruger, L., & Swanson, L. W. (2007). 1710: The introduction of experimental nervous system physiology and anatomy by Fransçois Pourfour du Petit. In H. Whitaker, C. U. M. Smith, & S. Finger (Eds.), Brain, mind and medicine: Essays in eighteenth century neuroscience (pp. 99–113). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lachman, S. J. (1963). History and methods of physiological psychology. Detroit, MI: The Hamilton Press.Google Scholar
  26. Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain mechanisms and intelligence. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  27. McGaugh, J. J. (2000). Memory—A century of consolidation. Science, 287, 248–251.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Misiak, H. (1961). The philosophical roots of scientific psychology. New York, NY: Fordham University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Murchison, C. (1929). The psychological register. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Neuberger, M. (1981). The historical development of experimental and spinal cord physiology before Flourens. (E. Clarke, Trans., Ed., Supplemented). Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Rosenzweig, M. R. (1959). Salivary conditioning before Pavlov. American Journal of Psychology, 72, 628–633.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Thomas, R. K. (1970). Mass function and equipotentiality: A reanalysis of Lashley’s retention data. Psychological Reports, 27, 899–902.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Thomas, R. K. (1999). Franz, Shepherd Ivory. In J. A. Garraty & M. C. Carnes (Eds.), American national biography (Vol. 8, pp. 405–406). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Thomas, R. K. (2000). Franz, Shepherd Ivory. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 396–398). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Thomas, R. K. (2007). Recurring errors among recent history of psychology textbooks. American Journal of Psychology, 120, 477–495.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Thomas, R. K. (2011, October). Shepherd I. Franz (1874-1933) and Karl S. Lashley (1890-1958): An example of unfair historical recognition? Invited presentation at the Center for History of Psychology, University of Akron, Akron, OH.Google Scholar
  37. Thomas, R. K. (2012). Flourens, Pierre. In R. W. Rieber (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the history of psychological theories (Part 6) (pp. 442–443). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  38. Thomas, R. K. (in press-a). Brain and intelligence. In H. L. Miller (Ed.). Encyclopedia of theory in psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.Google Scholar
  39. Thomas, R. K. (in press-b). Shepherd Ivory Franz (1874–1933). In J. L. Pate (Ed.), Southern society for philosophy and psychology presidents and presidential addresses. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Rodopi Press.Google Scholar
  40. Uttal, W. R. (2003). The new phrenology: The limits of localizing cognitive processes in the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  41. Windholz, G. (1997). Ivan P. Pavlov: An overview of his life and psychological work American Psychologist (Special issue, “Commemorating Pavlov’s Work”), 52, 941–946.Google Scholar
  42. Windholz, G., & Lamal, P. A. (1993). Vagaries of science: Priority, independent discovery, and the quest for recognition. The Psychological Record, 43, 339–350.Google Scholar
  43. Wolfe, R. J. (2001). Tarnished idol: William Thomas Green Morton and the introduction of surgical anesthesia, a chronicle of the ether controversy. San Anselmo, CA: Norman Publishing.Google Scholar
  44. Yakolev, P. I. (1959). Bechterev. In M. A. B. Brazier (Ed.), The central nervous system and behavior: Transaction of the first conference, February 23, 24, 25, and 26, 1958 (pp. 187–210). Madison NJ: Madison Printing.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of GeorgiaAthensUSA

Personalised recommendations