Advertisement

The Psychological Record

, Volume 64, Issue 3, pp 393–402 | Cite as

The Role of Correspondence Training on Children’s Self-Report Accuracy across Tasks

  • Mariéle Diniz Cortez
  • Julio C. de Rose
  • Caio F. Miguel
Original Article

Abstract

This study investigated children’s self-report accuracy as a function of task type and also verified generalization of do-say correspondence across tasks. Six children between 6 and 11 years of age participated in the study. “Doing” consisted of reading words, playing a computer game, solving a math problem, and labeling music-related stimuli. “Saying” consisted of reporting on the accuracy of performance following the automated computer feedback. Baseline assessed correspondence for the different tasks. Correspondence training was conducted for the task in which levels of accuracy were the lowest. Generalized do-say correspondence was then assessed in untrained tasks. For four children, correspondence was lowest for the academic tasks. Four of six children exhibited generalized correspondence after the first training, and the remaining two children did so following a second training with a different task. Distinct tasks seemed to control different patterns of self-report accuracy. Results on generalization indicated do-say correspondence as a generalized operant behavior.

Keywords

Correspondence training Do-say correspondence Generalization Lying Self-report 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by The National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq; Grant # 573972/2008-7), and by FAPESP (Grants # 08/57705-8 and # 08/50988-4). We would like to thank Danielle LaFrance for her suggestions on a previous version of this manuscript.

References

  1. Baer, R. A., & Detrich, R. (1990). Tacting and manding in correspondence training: effects of child selection of verbalization. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 23–30. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1990.54-23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baer, R. A., Osnes, P. G., & Stokes, T. F. (1983). Training generalized correspondence between verbal behavior at school and nonverbal behavior at home. Education and Treatment of Children, 6, 379–388.Google Scholar
  3. Baer, R. A., Williams, J. A., Osnes, P. G., & Stokes, T. F. (1984). Delayed reinforcement as an indiscriminable contingency in verbal/nonverbal correspondence training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, 429–440. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1984.17-429.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Baer, R. A., Williams, J. A., Osnes, P. G., & Stokes, T. F. (1985). Generalized verbal control and correspondence training. Behavior Modification, 9, 477–489. doi: 10.1177/01454455850094005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baer, R. A., Blount, R. L., Detrich, R., & Stokes, T. F. (1987). Using intermittent reinforcement to program maintenance of verbal/nonverbal correspondence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 179–184. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1987.20-179.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Baer, R. A., Detrich, R., & Weninger, J. M. (1988). On the functional role on the verbalization in correspondence training procedures. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21(4), 345–356. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1988.21-345.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Ballard, K. D., & Jenner, L. (1981). Establishing correspondence between saying and doing as a procedure for increasing social behaviors of two elementary school children. The Exceptional Child, 28, 55–63. doi: 10.1080/0156655810280106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Beckert, M. E. (2005). Correspondência verbal/não verbal: Pesquisa básica e aplicações na clínica. In J. Abreu-Rodrigues & M. Rodrigues Ribeiro (Eds.), Análise do Comportamento (pp. 229–244). Porto Alegre: Artmed.Google Scholar
  9. Bevill, A. R., Gast, D. L., Maguire, A. M., & Vail, C. O. (2001). Increasing engagement of preschoolers with disabilities through correspondence training and picture cues. Journal of Early Intervention, 24(2), 129–145. doi: 10.1177/105381510102400207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brino, A. L. F., & de Rose, J. C. C. (2006). Correspondência entre auto-relatos e desempenhos acadêmicos antecedentes em crianças com história de fracasso escolar. Revista Brasileira de Análise do Comportamento, 2(1), 67–77.Google Scholar
  11. Cortez, M. D., de Rose, J. C., & Montagnoli, T. A. S. (2013). Treino e manutenção de correspondência em autorrelatos de crianças com e sem história de fracasso escolar. Acta Comportamentalia, 21(2), 139–157.Google Scholar
  12. Critchfield, T. S., & Perone, M. (1990). Verbal self-reports of delayed matching to sample by humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 321–344. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1990.53-321.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Critchfield, T. S., & Perone, M. (1993). Verbal self-reports about matching to sample: effects of the number of elements in a compound sample stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 193–214. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1993.59-193.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Deacon, J. R., & Konarski, E. A. (1987). Correspondence training: an example of rule-governed behavior? Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 391–400. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1987.20-391.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Domeniconi, C, de Rose, J.C., & Perez, W.F. (2014). Effects of correspondence training on self-reports of errors during a reading task. The Psychological Record. doi: 10.1007/s40732-014-0009-z.
  16. Glenn, S. S. (1983). Maladaptive functional relations in client verbal behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 6, 47–56.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Greer, R. D., & Speckman, J. (2009). The integration of speaker and listener responses: a theory of verbal development. The Psychological Record, 59, 449–488.Google Scholar
  18. Guevremont, D. C., Osnes, P. G., & Stokes, T. F. (1986a). Preparation for effective self-regulation: the development of generalized verbal control. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 99–104. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1986.19-99.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Guevremont, D. C., Osnes, P. G., & Stokes, T. F. (1986b). Programming maintenance after correspondence training interventions with children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 215–219. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1986.19-215.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Israel, A. C. (1978). Some thoughts on correspondence between saying and doing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11(2), 271–276. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1978.11-271.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Karlan, G. R., & Rusch, F. R. (1982). Correspondence between saying and doing: some thoughts on defining correspondence and future directions for application. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 151–162. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1982.15-151.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Lima, E. L., & Abreu-Rodrigues, J. (2010). Verbal mediating responses: effects on generalization of say-do correspondence and noncorrespondence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 411–424. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2010.43-411.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Lloyd, K. E. (2002). A review of correspondence training: suggestions for a revival. The Behavior Analyst, 25, 57–73.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Luciano, M. C., Herruzo, J., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2001). Generalization of say-do correspondence. The Psychological Record, 51, 111–130.Google Scholar
  25. Luciano-Soriano, M. C., Molina-Cobos, F. J., & Gómez-Becerra, I. (2000). Say-do-report training to change chronic behaviors in mentally retarded subjects. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21(5), 355–366. doi: 10.1016/S0891-4222(00)00048-2.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Machalicek, W., Shogren, K., Lang, R., Rispoli, M., O’Reilly, M. F., Franco, J. H., et al. (2009). Increasing play and decreasing the challenging behavior of children with autism during recess with activity schedules and task corresponding training. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(2), 547–555. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2008.11.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Michael, J. (2004). Concepts and principles of behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Kalamazoo, MI: Association for Behavior Analysis International.Google Scholar
  28. Morrison, R. S., Sainato, D. M., Benchaaban, D., & Endo, S. (2002). Increasing play skills of children with autism using activity schedules and correspondence training. Journal of Early Intervention, 25(1), 58–72. doi: 10.1177/105381510202500106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Paniagua, F. A. (1989). Lying by children: why children say one thing, do another? Psychological Reports, 64, 971–984.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Ribeiro, A. F. (1989). Correspondence in children’s self-report: tacting and manding aspects. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 361–367. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1989.51-361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Risley, T., & Hart, B. (1968). Developing correspondence between nonverbal and verbal behavior of preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 267–281. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1968.1-267.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Roca, J. V., & Gross, A. M. (1996). Report-do-report: promoting setting and setting-time generalization. Education and Treatment of Children, 19, 408–424.Google Scholar
  33. Rosa Filho, A.B.., de Rose, J.C.C., de Souza, D.G., Hanna, E.S., & Fonseca, M.L. (1998). Aprendendo a ler e escrever em pequenos passos. Software para pesquisa.Google Scholar
  34. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Stein, L. M. (1994). TDE – Teste de Desempenho Escolar: Manual para aplicação e interpretação. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo.Google Scholar
  36. Stokes, J. V., Cameron, M. J., Dorsey, M. F., & Fleming, E. (2004). Task analysis, correspondence training, and general case instruction for teaching personal hygiene skills. Behavior Interventions, 19, 121–135. doi: 10.1002/bin.153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ward, W. D., & Stare, S. W. (1990). The role of subject verbalization in generalized correspondence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 129–136. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1990.23-129.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Watson, P. J., & Workman, E. A. (1981). The non-concurrent multiple baseline across-individuals design: An extension of the traditional multiple baseline design. Journal of Behavior and Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 12, 257–259. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(81)90055-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Williams, J. A., & Stokes, T. G. (1982). Some parameters of correspondence training and generalized verbal control. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 4, 11–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wilson, P. G., Rusch, F. R., & Lee, S. (1992). Strategies to increase exercise-report correspondence by boys with moderate mental retardation: collateral changes in intention-exercise correspondence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 681–690. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1992.25-681.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mariéle Diniz Cortez
    • 1
    • 2
  • Julio C. de Rose
    • 1
    • 2
    • 4
  • Caio F. Miguel
    • 3
  1. 1.Universidade Federal de São CarlosSão CarlosBrazil
  2. 2.Instituto Nacional de Ciência e Tecnologia sobre Comportamento, Cognição e EnsinoSão CarlosBrazil
  3. 3.California State UniversitySacramentoUSA
  4. 4.Departamento de PsicologiaSão CarlosBrazil

Personalised recommendations