Introduction

The adult animal possesses a fine-tuned sensorimotor system. Fast and efficient integration of motor commands and their resulting movements with associated sensory consequences is necessary for virtually every action an animal makes. The act of grasping a pencil, for example, requires a constant, bidirectional flow of information between the sensory and motor systems. And yet, despite the obvious importance of this sensorimotor integration, we know relatively little about where it comes from. A fine-tuned sensorimotor system cannot simply be endowed or preprogrammed; it must develop, and it must do so within the context of a continually and often rapidly changing body, with limb sizes and biomechanics changing from one day to the next [1]. How, then, do sensorimotor networks adapt to these changes? What developing animals do while awake certainly matters. Here, we explore the ways in which movements during sleep might matter, too.

Young animals spend the majority of their time asleep. For example, human newborns typically sleep for 14–16 h a day, of which half is spent in REM (or active) sleep [24]. Conventionally, sleep is thought of as a time of behavioral quiescence, but this could not be further from the truth, especially during early development. Indeed, skeletal muscles throughout the body twitch frequently during active sleep [5]. As muscles twitch and limbs move, tactile receptors and proprioceptors transmit signals to the brain (e.g., [6, 7]). Recent research suggests that this sensorimotor experience helps to drive activity-dependent development of the sensorimotor system [8, 9].

Here, we review three aspects of twitching that provide insight into their role in sensorimotor development. We begin with the behavioral expression of twitches and their spatiotemporal structure. Next, we describe how twitches activate the central nervous system; we also describe how sensory feedback is processed differently during sleep than during wakefulness and how this difference may be necessary for twitches to contribute to sensorimotor development. Lastly, we suggest that studying twitching may help us understand better the etiology of certain neurodevelopmental disorders.

An Ideal Movement

Twitches of the limbs are first expressed in the late fetal period in rats [10, 11] and can still be observed in adults [12], albeit at a reduced frequency. In rats, the highest amount of twitching can be observed during the first two postnatal weeks [13], which coincides with a period of rapid growth [1] and the emergence of adult-like motor skills [14]. Twitches occur in all skeletal muscles that have been investigated thus far, including those that control the limbs and digits [5], eyes [15], and whiskers [16], and it has been estimated that newborn rats produce hundreds of thousands of twitches each day [17]. Because motor learning requires copious amounts of experience [18], the sheer quantity of twitches suggests that they serve an important role.

Quantity matters, but so does quality. Twitches are produced in a state that allows them to be uniquely discrete and salient. Produced only during active sleep, twitches occur when all skeletal muscles are atonic [19]. Also, although twitches can occur in rapid succession, they rarely happen simultaneously [5], further reinforcing their discrete nature. In contrast, wake movements are typically composed of continuous and simultaneous muscle activations. If twitches are to provide information about the body to develop, refine, and maintain sensorimotor maps [9, 17, 20], then the singular event of a discrete twitch would seem to be well suited to contributing to those processes. It is much easier to pay attention to a single event when all else is quiet than when that single event is drowned out by a sea of extraneous activity.

Twitches occur so rapidly and abruptly, and at such low amplitudes compared to wake movements, that, to the naked eye, they appear to be randomly generated movements with no observable structure. High-speed videography combined with three-dimensional motion tracking has proven to be a powerful tool to reveal the intricate kinematics of twitches and how they change across early development [5]. When captured at 250 frames per second, what initially looks like a single twitch of the forelimb amidst a bout of twitching reveals itself as just one piece of a complex, sequential activation of twitches at multiple joints within and between limbs. Indeed, as early as 2 days of age, rats exhibit such synergistic patterns of twitching.

Furthermore, in the same way that synapses compete amongst themselves for access to postsynaptic sites, patterns of twitching appear to compete for retention and expression during the early postnatal period [5]. That the structure of twitches can be shaped by developmental experience suggests an underlying change in the spinal and brainstem mechanisms that control them. More broadly, they suggest that twitching contributes to the fine-tuning of the sensorimotor system as a whole, thus influencing the development and expression of movements during wake.

The value of twitching is not limited to biological systems. Despite incredible and rapid advances in modern robotics, we are still unable to create a robot that can learn and adapt in the real world. Some roboticists see this problem as a limitation inherent in the way that robots come into being: in short, they do not develop and therefore never experience—and never learn to adapt to—the variability and vicissitudes of a growing body [21]. In taking this idea seriously, developmental roboticists created a model of the musculoskeletal system, complete with neural circuitry, and provided it with the ability to “twitch” and update its circuitry based on sensory feedback [22, 23]. Equipped in this way, the system was able to self-organize its neural circuitry, including circuits that resemble those underlying the most basic spinal reflexes. This mimicking of biological processes within a robotic environment holds great promise for testing hypotheses regarding the control and functions of twitching in animals.

The above-described features of twitches are indicative of an autonomous mechanism that is capable of providing the animal with a sizeable amount of experience to learn about its body throughout development. Additionally, this experience is organized and structured in a way that can change from one day to the next as the animal develops its repertoire of movements. However, for the organism to benefit from the experience provided by twitches, the brain must receive this information and process it in a meaningful way.

An Abundance of Sensory Feedback

Despite their discrete nature and low amplitude, twitches readily activate the brain. In fact, in response to twitches, bouts of neural activity are triggered throughout the brain, including the brainstem [24], thalamus [16], hippocampus [6], and primary somatosensory and motor cortices [68]. Arguably, twitches are a primary activator of neural activity in the developing brain. This neural activity is phasic, displaying short bursts of action potentials that follow the onset of a twitch, indicative of sensory feedback. Furthermore, neural responses exhibit different latencies from twitch onset, depending on the distance that the feedback signal must travel and the number of interposed synapses.

The short bursts of action potentials that follow each twitch result in strong correlations between twitching and neural activity [6, 8, 16]. Such strong correlations result from the discrete nature of twitches within a low-noise environment. It has been suggested that correlation-based learning associated with twitching contributes to the self-organization of spinal circuits underlying the withdrawal reflex [9]. This form of learning could also be important for the development and maintenance of somatotopic maps.

To further explore the role of twitching in sensorimotor development, we turned our attention to the cerebellum, a structure that plays a critical role in sensorimotor integration [25] and undergoes tremendous growth in rats over the first few weeks of postnatal life [26, 27]. Postnatal changes in organization and synaptic connectivity in the cerebellum have been proposed to be activity dependent [2830]. Therefore, if twitches activate cells within the cerebellar circuitry, as observed in other brain areas, they could provide precise information and thereby facilitate the postnatal development and refinement of this sensorimotor structure.

We focused on Purkinje cells, the sole output of the cerebellar cortex. At birth, the Purkinje cell layer is already established [26] and neural activity can be measured by the third postnatal day in rats [31, 32]. Purkinje cells receive two primary inputs, climbing fibers and mossy fibers. Climbing fibers arise from the inferior olive, and mossy fibers arise from a number of brainstem nuclei, including the lateral reticular nucleus and pontine gray [33]. In adults, each Purkinje cell receives input from only one climbing fiber; in contrast, each Purkinje cell receives multiple mossy fiber inputs via the granule cell-parallel fiber pathway. Early in the postnatal period, however, each Purkinje cell is innervated by multiple climbing fibers and the granule cell-parallel fiber pathway is undeveloped, with mossy fibers forming direct, transient connections with Purkinje cells [28, 34, 35]. Because development of the cerebellar system continues into the third postnatal week, it is a potentially powerful model system for assessing the role of twitching on activity-dependent developmental processes.

Purkinje cells show two distinct firing patterns based on input from either climbing fibers or mossy fibers: complex spikes and simple spikes, respectively. Although both complex spikes and simple spikes are produced in response to peripheral stimulation in young rats, the majority of Purkinje cell activity in the developing cerebellum has been thought to be spontaneous and random [32, 36]. However, in week-old rats, we found that 60 % of Purkinje cells exhibited state dependency. Of these Purkinje cells, 65 % exhibited more activity during active sleep than during wakefulness [37]. Moreover, we found strong correlations between the onset of a twitch and the onset of complex and simple spikes; twitches preceded both complex and simple spikes. These observations promise to help us understand the contributions of twitching to the development of the cerebellum and its complex interactions with other structures, including the neocortex [38], as well as the role of the cerebellum in the development of motor synergies.

The Sleeping Brain Processes Sensory Information in a Unique Way

In retrospect, it is perhaps not surprising that twitches readily activate the cerebellum, thalamus, cerebral cortex, and hippocampus. But it was surprising that, time and time again, we noted considerably less neural activity in these same structures when pups were awake and vigorously moving their limbs. What could explain such puzzling observations? Could there be differences in sensorimotor processing during sleep and wake? To answer this question, we must first review some basic features of sensorimotor processing.

The generation of movements begins with the transmission of motor commands from motor areas in the brain and spinal cord to the muscles (Fig. 1a). When a movement occurs, sensory receptors in the moving limb transmit information back to the brain. Also, at the same time that the motor command is generated, a motor copy, or corollary discharge, is produced and transmitted to non-motor areas [39]. This corollary discharge signal is then compared to the sensory feedback produced by the associated movement. If the movement happens as expected, and no unknown forces disturb the planned movement (i.e., the limb does not collide unexpectedly with an external object), the sensory feedback will match the corollary discharge signal, informing the brain that no unexpected outcome occurred. On the other hand, if the limb does collide unexpectedly with an external object, the resulting sensory feedback cannot be matched to the corollary discharge signal. Because of this mismatch, feedback is relayed to downstream sensory areas, informing the brain about the unexpected outcome. Using this mechanism, organisms are able to understand that the movements they make are indeed their own [40].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Hypothetical pathways depicting the sensorimotor neural networks of wake-related and sleep-related movements. a Conventional model representing wake-related self-generated movements. Motor systems generate motor commands that activate muscles, thereby eliciting sensory reafference (green arrow). At the same time, motor systems generate a corollary discharge (red arrow). This corollary discharge is then compared to reafference, which can result in modulation of the neural feedback before it reaches other downstream sensory areas (black arrow). b Proposed model for the neural circuitry involved in the generation and processing of twitches. Similar to other self-generated movements, motor systems generate motor commands that activate muscles, thereby eliciting sensory reafference (curved green arrow). Unique to twitches, however, the motor systems do not generate a simultaneously produced corollary discharge, or alternatively, the corollary discharge is generated but its effects are inhibited. Because of this, reafference is not compared to corollary discharge and is therefore minimally modulated on its way to downstream sensory areas (short green arrow). Adapted from Tiriac et al., Current Biology, 2014

So after repeatedly observing very little neural activity when wake movements were produced, we hypothesized that corollary discharge mechanisms might be canceling or gating the sensory feedback [37]. We further hypothesized that twitches, although self-generated, are processed by the brain as if they are unexpected (Fig. 1b). To test these hypotheses, we contrived various experimental paradigms in which infant rats produced expected (i.e., in which corollary discharge is presumably recruited) and unexpected (i.e., in which corollary discharge is presumably not recruited) self-generated movements. If the mechanisms of corollary discharge filter out sensory feedback arising from expected movements, we predicted that only the unexpected self-generated movements would trigger neural activity, thereby mimicking what was observed with twitches.

To produce unexpected self-generated movements, we activated motorneurons directly in the spinal cord while recording from primary motor cortex (M1). We chose to record from M1 because, contrary to its name, it processes both sensory [41] and motor activity during early development [42]. By directly activating the motorneurons, we bypassed motor circuitry in the brain and spinal cord where corollary discharge signals are likely generated [43, 44]. Initially, we used the serotonin agonist quipazine, a drug known to directly activate spinal motorneurons to induce stepping [45]. As we predicted, animals injected with quipazine (but not saline) exhibited vigorous movements of the hindlimbs that were reminiscent of waking movements but, in contrast with waking movements, sensory feedback from the movements was robustly relayed to M1.

Next, we designed two methods to evoke expected and unexpected self-generated movements in the same subjects, thereby allowing us to directly compare the effects of the two types of movements on M1 activity. To produce an unexpected self-generated movement, we flicked the tail of the animal, thereby triggering a spinal reflex that in turn elicited a hindlimb movement. We assumed that this tail-to-hindlimb spinal reflex circuitry would not recruit corollary discharge mechanisms. To produce an expected self-generated movement, we applied a cold stimulus to the snout of the pup, thereby causing global arousal and vigorous hindlimb movements. Because this method activated motor networks in the brain, we assumed that it would recruit corollary discharge mechanisms. Once again, as predicted, only the unexpected movements caused by flicks of the tail triggered robust activity in M1.

All together, these results led us to infer that twitching is a self-generated movement that is processed as if it is unexpected. To our knowledge, this is the first such behavior to exhibit this characteristic. Importantly, this finding has intriguing functional implications. Specifically, under normal waking conditions, the filtering of sensory feedback by corollary discharge mechanisms makes functional sense. But for a developing infant that relies on activity-dependent mechanisms for the development of neural networks, the engagement of corollary discharge mechanisms and the resulting filtering of sensory feedback would be counterproductive—such filtering would deprive the pup of important sensory information about the structure and biomechanical properties of its limbs. Therefore, in showing that twitches differ from wake movements on this critical dimension, we have provided additional support for the notion that they participate in a unique way in the development of the sensorimotor system.

Sensorimotor Deficiencies and Neurodevelopmental Disorders

If we think of our various sensorimotor, cognitive, and emotional systems as modular and encapsulated, then there is no harm in studying them separately. But such modular thinking, we believe, is misguided. Cognition is embodied [46]; therefore, we should not be surprised by the fact that people with severe mental illness (e.g., schizophrenia, autism) typically exhibit pronounced sensorimotor deficits. The studies described in this review address the role that sensorimotor processes during sleep play in the activation of the infant brain. In that context, it seems reasonable to suggest that any genetic or environmental factor that disrupts twitching and its effects on brain activity could, in turn, disrupt downstream developmental processes that depend on sensorimotor processing. In other words, sensorimotor dysfunction in early development could drive later-emerging cognitive and emotional problems. It follows that a thorough understanding of sleep and twitching in early development may prove useful for the early detection and treatment of mental illness.

For example, our recent findings regarding corollary discharge mechanisms in early infancy [7] open new avenues for research into the mechanisms of state-dependent modulation of the sensorimotor system in health and disease. Of particular interest here is accumulating evidence that people with schizophrenia exhibit deficits in the functioning of their corollary discharge system, potentially explaining such positive symptoms as hallucinations and delusions of control [47]. Our methods and concepts could easily be exported to investigations of sensorimotor processing in children at risk for schizophrenia.

As another example, consider the well-established links between early cerebellar damage and autism, as well as “evidence that the cerebellum may guide the maturation of remote non-motor neural circuitry and influence cognitive development” [48]. Indeed, the cerebellum has been implicated in a variety of cognitive processes and mental disorders [49]. In light of our recent demonstration of cerebellar activation by sleep-related twitching [37], it seems plausible that active sleep is an important moderator of cerebellar development and the downstream consequences of alterations in that development. For example, as shown many years ago in infant rodents, twitching is very sensitive to changes in the thermal environment [50], which may help to explain the link between extreme environmental deprivation and the development of autism-like characteristics [51].

Conclusions

Twitches appear ideally suited to instruct the brain about the rapidly developing body. On the motor side, twitches are produced in a non-random, organized fashion, and complex patterns of twitches are selectively enhanced or eliminated throughout development. In other words, the structure and pattern of twitching is organized but flexible. Additionally, unlike the continuous movements occurring during wakefulness, twitches are produced discretely against a background of muscle atonia, providing a high signal-to-noise ratio. On the sensory side, early in development, twitches are a primary activator of neural networks throughout the central nervous system. The sheer quantity of neural activity resulting from twitches alone provides a sizeable amount of sensory experience at ages when activity-dependent development is so important [52]. Lastly, it is imperative that sensory information arriving from the periphery is transmitted with high fidelity, which is made possible by the apparent lack of corollary discharge mechanisms during twitching.

The findings described here provide evidence that strongly implicates twitching as playing an important and unique role in sensorimotor development. The fact that twitches occur so prevalently during developmental periods associated with accelerated growth [1], reflex integration [9], and locomotor development [14] seems unlikely to be a mere coincidence. On the contrary, the evidence seems to be pointing the other way, in favor of the notion that active sleep and its associated twitching during early development were selected over long periods of evolution for their ability to provide animals with an effective system for learning about their bodies.