Advertisement

Behavior Analysis in Practice

, Volume 11, Issue 4, pp 411–416 | Cite as

The Mediating Effects of Derived Relational Responding on the Relationship Between Verbal Operant Development and IQ

  • Jordan Belisle
  • Mark R. DixonEmail author
  • Caleb R. Stanley
Brief Practice

Abstract

Dixon, Belisle, and Stanley, (2018) demonstrated a strong, significant relationship between derived relational responding and intelligence in individuals with autism. We extended these results by evaluating the degree to which participant results on the PEAK Equivalence Pre-assessment (PEAK-E-PA, a direct assessment of derived relational responding) mediated the relationship between the PEAK Direct Training Assessment (PEAK-DT-A, a Skinnerian-based assessment of verbal development) and intelligence. Results support strong, positive correlations between both assessments (PEAK-DT-A and PEAK-E-PA) and IQ; however, the relationship between PEAK-DT-A and IQ could be explained in terms of participant results on the PEAK-E-PA alone. This finding corresponds with Relational Frame Theory, suggesting that derived responding can provide a behavioral interpretation of intelligent behavior, as well as Skinner’s elementary operants.

Keywords

Derived relational responding RFT Intelligence Autism 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from the participants, agencies, and/or legal guardians of all participants included in this study.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Conflict of Interest

The second author receives small royalties from sales of the PEAK curriculum. The other authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A relational frame training intervention to raise intelligence quotients: A pilot study. The Psychological Record, 61, 173–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Dixon, M. R. (2014). PEAK relational training system: Direct training module. Carbondale: Shawnee Scientific Press.Google Scholar
  3. Dixon, M. R. (2015). PEAK relational training system: Equivalence module. Carbondale: Shawnee Scientific Press.Google Scholar
  4. Dixon, M. R., Belisle, J., Blevins, A., & Hayes, S. C. (under review). Derived relational responding is a generalized operant: Evidence from children with autism using the PEAK-E curriculum.Google Scholar
  5. Dixon, M. R., Belisle, J., & Stanley, C. R. (2018). Derived relational responding and intelligence: Assessing the relationship between the PEAK-E Pre-Assessment and IQ with individuals with disabilities. The Psychological Record.Google Scholar
  6. Dixon, M. R., Stanley, C. R., Belisle, J., & Rowsey, K. E. (2016). The test-retest and interrater reliability of the promoting the emergence of advanced knowledge-direct training assessment for use with individuals with autism and related disabilities. Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice, 16, 34–40.  https://doi.org/10.1037/bar0000027.Google Scholar
  7. Dixon, M. R., Whiting, S. W., & Daar, J. H. (2014). Introduction. In M. R. Dixon (Ed.), PEAK relational training system: Direct training module. Carbondale: Shawnee Scientific Press.Google Scholar
  8. Dixon, M. R., Whiting, S. W., Rowsey, K., & Belisle, J. (2014). Assessing the relationship between intelligence and the PEAK relational training system. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 1208–1213.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.05.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  10. Healy, O., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (2000). Derived relational responding as generalized operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 207–227.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2000.74-207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Sattler, J. M., & Dumont, R. (2004). Assessment of children: WISC-IV and WPPSI-III supplement. San Diego: Sattler Publisher, Inc..Google Scholar
  12. Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1982.37-5.
  13. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence”. Objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 238–249.Google Scholar
  15. Sundberg, M. L. (2008). VB-MAPP: Verbal behavior milestones assessment and placement program. Concord: AVB Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jordan Belisle
    • 1
  • Mark R. Dixon
    • 1
    Email author
  • Caleb R. Stanley
    • 1
  1. 1.Rehabilitation InstituteSouthern Illinois UniversityCarbondaleUSA

Personalised recommendations