The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 32, Issue 2, pp 242–264 | Cite as

The Effects of Blocking and Joint Control Training on Sequencing Visual Stimuli

  • Curtis W. Clough
  • Careen S. Meyer
  • Caio F. Miguel
Article

Abstract

We assessed the effects of blocking on the accuracy of arranging visual stimuli in sequences as an attempt to assess whether verbal behavior mediates nonverbal performance. Across three experiments, college students were trained to echo and tact the names of abstract images vocally (Experiments 1 and 3) and with hand signs (Experiment 2), and then, they were tested to see whether they could sequence these pictures accurately in the presence of their dictated names or signs. When participants were required to engage in a vocal blocking task, sequencing performances learned via either vocal or hand signs deteriorated (Experiments 1 and 2). In Experiment 3, vocal blocking deteriorated sequencing learned vocally, but not when participants were responding to visual samples (i.e., visual matching). Overall, only 2 out of 12 participants required joint control training to accurately sequence stimuli. Combined results suggest that vocal blocking may serve to prevent verbal behavior that could be mediating sequencing, and that joint control training is not necessary for adults to perform the sequencing task.

Keywords

Blocking Echoic Joint control Tact Verbal behavior 

References

  1. Causin, K. G., Albert, K. M., Carbone, V. J., & Sweeney-Kerwin, E. J. (2013). The role of joint control in teaching listener responding to children with autism and other developmental disabilities. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 997–1011. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2013.04.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Coon, J. T., & Miguel, C. F. (2012). The role of increased exposure to transfer-of-stimulus-control procedures on the acquisition of intraverbal behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(4), 657–666. do: 10.1901/jaba.2012.45-657.
  3. Cumming, W. W., & Berryman, R. (1961). Some data on matching behavior in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 281–284. doi:10.1901/jeab.1961.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. DeGraaf, A., & Schlinger, H. (2012). The effect of joint control training on the acquisition and durability of a sequencing task. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 28, 59–71.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Gutierrez, R. D. (2006). The role of rehearsal in joint control. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 183–190.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Horne, P. J., Hughes, J. C., & Lowe, C. F. (2006). Naming and categorization in young children: IV: listener behavior training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 247–273. doi:10.1901/jeab.2006.125-04.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241. doi:10.1901/jeab.2004.81-267.
  8. Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Harris, F. D. A. (2007). Naming and categorization in young children: V. Manual sign training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 367–381. doi:10.1901/jeab.2007.52-06.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Randle, V. R. L. (2004). Naming and categorization in young children: II. Listener behavior training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 267–288. doi:10.1901/jeab.2004.81-267.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Kobari-Wright, V. V., & Miguel, C. F. (2014). The effects of listener training on the emergence of categorization and speaker behavior in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, 431–436. doi:10.1002/jaba.115.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., Harris, F. D. A., & Randle, V. R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young children: vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527–549. doi:10.1901/jeab.2002.78-527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Lowenkron, B. (1984). Coding responses and the generalization of matching to sample in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 1–18. doi:10.1901/jeab.1984.42-1.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Lowenkron, B. (1988). Generalization of delayed identity matching in retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 163–172. doi:10.1901/jeab.1988.50-163.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Lowenkron, B. (1989). Instructional control of generalized relational matching to sample in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 293–309. doi:10.1901/jeab.1989.52-293.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Lowenkron, B. (1991). Joint control and the generalization of selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 121–126.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical functions of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354. doi:10.1901/jeab.1998.69-327.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Lowenkron, B. (2004). Meaning: a verbal behavior account. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 20, 77–97.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Lowenkron, B. (2006a). An introduction to joint control. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 123–127.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Lowenkron, B. (2006b). Joint control and the selection of stimuli from their description. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 129–151.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Lowenkron, B., & Colvin, V. (1992). Joint control and generalized nonidentity matching: saying when something is not. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 1–10.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Lowenkron, B., & Colvin, V. (1995). ‘Generalized instructional control and the production of broadly applicable relational responding’: erratum. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 12, 13–29.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Ma, M. L., Miguel, C. F., & Jennings, A. M. (2016). Training intraverbal naming to establish equivalence class performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 105, 409–426. doi:10.1002/jeab.203.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Mahoney, A. M., Miguel, C. F., Ahearn, W. H., & Bell, J. (2011). The role of common motor responses in stimulus categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 237–262. doi:10.1901/jeab.2011.95-237.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Miguel, C. F. (2016). Common and intraverbal bidirectional naming. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. doi:10.1007/s40616-016-0066-2.
  25. Miguel, C. F., & Kobari-Wright, V. V. (2013). The effects of tact training on the emergence of categorization and listener behavior in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 669–673. doi:10.1002/jaba.62.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., & Michael, J. (2008). The role of naming in stimulus categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 383–405. doi:10.1901/jeab.2008-89-383.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Palmer, D. C. (2006). Joint control: a discussion of recent research. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 209–215.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Petursdottir, A. I., Ólafsdóttir, A. R., & Aradóttir, B. (2008). The effects of tact and listener training on the emergence of bidirectional intraverbal relations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 411–415. doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-411.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Ratkos, T., Frider, J. E., & Poling, A. (2016). Accurate delayed matching-to-sample responding without rehearsal: an unintentional demonstration with children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 32, 66–77. doi:10.1007/s4061-016-0052-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Santos, P. M., Ma, M. L., & Miguel, C. F. (2015). Training intraverbal naming to establish matching-to-sample performances. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 31, 162–182. doi:10.1007/s40616-015-0040-4.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Sidener, D. W., & Michael, J. (2006). Generalization of relational matching to sample in children: a direct replication. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 171–181.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Sindelar, P., Rosenberg, M., & Wilson, R. (1985). An adapted alternating treatments design for instructional research. Education and Treatment of Children, 8, 67–76.Google Scholar
  33. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton: Copley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Tu, J. C. (2006). The role of joint control in the manded selection responses of both vocal and non-vocal children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 191–207.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Watson, P. J., & Workman, E. A. (1981). The nonconcurrent multiple baseline across individuals design: an extension of the traditional multiple baseline design. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 12, 257–259. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(81)90055-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Curtis W. Clough
    • 1
  • Careen S. Meyer
    • 1
  • Caio F. Miguel
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyCalifornia State University, SacramentoSacramentoUSA

Personalised recommendations