Advertisement

A Neuro-Operant Analysis of Mnemonic Recognition

  • Daniele Ortu
  • Traci M. Cihon
Article
  • 55 Downloads

Abstract

Historically, the fields of operant selection and recognition memory have not interacted substantially with one another. However, both deal with how behavioral repertoires change over time as a function of environmental stimulation. In this article, we propose neuro-operant interpretations of behavioral phenomena occurring in recognition memory procedures based on (a) the ability to discriminate changes in the strength of responses caused by environmental stimulation and (b) the occasioning of supplementary responses by current stimulation. A neuro-operant interpretation of mnemonic behavior may further the understanding of the phenomena in place and simplify the current taxonomy of learning and memory.

Keywords

Hippocampus Mnemonic behavior Operant selection Perirhinal cortex Priming Repertoire 

Notes

Funding

Daniele Ortu is funded by the Beatrice H. Barrett Research Endowment.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aggleton, J. P., Vann, S. D., Denby, C., Dix, S., Mayes, A. R., Roberts, N., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2005). Sparing of the familiarity component of recognition memory in a patient with hippocampal pathology. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1810–1823.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.019.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baylis, G. C., & Rolls, E. T. (1987). Responses of neurons in the inferior temporal cortex in short term and serial recognition memory tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 65, 614–622.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00235984.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Becher, T., & Trowler, P. (2001). Academic tribes and territories: intellectual enquiry and the culture of disciplines. Buckingham, England: McGraw-Hill Education.Google Scholar
  4. Benoit, S. C., & Davidson, T. L. (1996). Interoceptive sensory signals produced by 24-hr food deprivation, pharmacological glucoprivation, and lipoprivation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, 168–180.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.110.1.168.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Berridge, C. W., & Waterhouse, B. D. (2003). The locus coeruleus–noradrenergic system: modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Research Reviews, 42, 33–84.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00143-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bowles, B., Crupi, C., Mirsattari, S. M., Pigott, S. E., Parrent, A. G., Pruessner, J. C.,. .. Köhler, S. (2007). Impaired familiarity with preserved recollection after anterior temporal-lobe resection that spares the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 16382–16387.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0705273104.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Whalen, P. J., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L.,. .. Rosen, B. R. (1996). Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron, 17, 875–887.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80219-6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Bussey, T. J., & Saksida, L. M. (2007). Memory, perception, and the ventral visual-perirhinal-hippocampal stream: thinking outside of the boxes. Hippocampus, 17, 898–908.  https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20320.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Buzsaki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cansino, S., Maquet, P., Dolan, R. J., & Rugg, M. D. (2002). Brain activity underlying encoding and retrieval of source memory. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 1048–1056.  https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/12.10.1048.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Carlesimo, G. A., Lombardi, M. G., Caltagirone, C., & Barban, F. (2015). Recollection and familiarity in the human thalamus. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 54, 18–28.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Donahoe, J. W. (1996). On the relation between behavior analysis and biology. The Behavior Analyst, 19, 71–73.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Donahoe, J. W. (2017). Behavior analysis and neuroscience: complementary disciplines. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 107, 301–320.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.251.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Donahoe, J. W., Burgos, J. E., & Palmer, D. C. (1993). A selectionist approach to reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 17–40.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1993.60-17.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Eichenbaum, H., Fortin, N. J., Ergorul, C., Wright, S. P., & Agster, K. L. (2005). Episodic recollection in animals: “If it walks like a duck and quacks like a duck . . .”. Learning and Motivation, 36, 190–207.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2005.02.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 30, 123–152.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ferster, C. B. (1953). The use of the free operant in the analysis of behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 50, 263–274.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0055514.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fischer, H., Wright, C. I., Whalen, P. J., McInerney, S. C., Shin, L. M., & Rauch, S. L. (2003). Brain habituation during repeated exposure to fearful and neutral faces: a functional MRI study. Brain Research Bulletin, 59, 387–392.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-9230(02)00940-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Fortin, N. J., Wright, S. P., & Eichenbaum, H. (2004). Recollection-like memory retrieval in rats is dependent on the hippocampus. Nature, 431, 188–191.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02853.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Friedman, D. (1990). ERPs during continuous recognition memory for words. Biological Psychology, 30, 61–87.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(90)90091-A.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Fryling, M. J., & Hayes, L. J. (2010). An interbehavioral analysis of memory. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11, 53–68.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2010.11434334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gimbel, S. I., Brewer, J. B., & Maril, A. (2017). I know I’ve seen you before: distinguishing recent-single-exposure-based familiarity from pre-existing familiarity. Brain Research, 1658, 11–24.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2017.01.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Glenn, S. S., Ellis, J., & Greenspoon, J. (1992). On the revolutionary nature of the operant as a unit of behavioral selection. American Psychologist, 47, 1329–1336.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.47.11.1329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gonsalves, B. D., Kahani, I., Curran, T., Norman, K. A., & Wagner, A. D. (2005). Memory strength and repetition suppression: multimodal imaging of medial temporal cortical contributions to recognition. Neuron, 47, 751–761.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.013.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2001). fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. Acta Psychologica, 107(1–3), 293–321.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00019-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Harris, J. D. (1943). Habituatory response decrement in the intact organism. Psychological Bulletin, 40, 385–422.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0053918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Henson, R. N. A. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of priming. Progress in Neurobiology, 70, 53–81.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(03)00086-8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Henson, R. N. A., & Rugg, M. D. (2003). Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming. Neuropsychologia, 41, 263–270.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00159-8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Horner, A. J., Bisby, J. A., Bush, D., Lin, W. J., & Burgess, N. (2015). Evidence for holistic episodic recollection via hippocampal pattern completion. Nature Communications, 6, 1–11.  https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms8462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2008). Priming, response learning and repetition suppression. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1979–1991.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.018.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Hull, D. L., Langman, R. E., & Glenn, S. S. (2001). A general account of selection: biology, immunology, and behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 511–528.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In R. Church & B. Campbell (Eds.), Punishment and aversive behavior (pp. 279–296). New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  34. Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1, 99–113.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073908100432.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Kesner, R. P., Lee, I., & Gilbert, P. (2004). A behavioral assessment of hippocampal function based on a subregional analysis. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15, 333–352.  https://doi.org/10.1515/REVNEURO.2004.15.5.333.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Lavenex, P., & Amaral, D. G. (2000). Hippocampal-neocortical interaction: a hierarchy of associativity. Hippocampus, 10, 420–430.  https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4<420::AID-HIPO8>3.0.CO;2-5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: the judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87, 252–271.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Michael, J., Palmer, D. C., & Sundberg, M. L. (2011). The multiple control of verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 3–22.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393089.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Muñoz-López, M., Insausti, R., Mohedano-Moriano, A., Mishkin, M., & Saunders, R. C. (2015). Anatomical pathways for auditory memory II: information from rostral superior temporal gyrus to dorsolateral temporal pole and medial temporal cortex. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2015.00158.
  40. Murty, V. P., & Adcock, R. A. (2013). Enriched encoding: reward motivation organizes cortical networks for hippocampal detection of unexpected events. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 2160–2168.  https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht063.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Neely, J. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: a selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  42. Neuman, K. M., Molina-Campos, E., Musial, T. F., Price, A. L., Oh, K. J., Wolke, M. L., … Nicholson, D. A. (2015). Evidence for Alzheimer’s disease-linked synapse loss and compensation in mouse and human hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Brain Structure and Function, 220, 3143–3165.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0848-z.
  43. Ortu, D. (2012). Neuroscientific measures of covert behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 35, 75–87.  https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03392267.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Ortu, D. (2015). How do we remember traumatic events? Exploring the role of neuromodulation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e19.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000260.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Ortu, D., & Vaidya, M. (2013). A neurobiology of learning beyond the declarative non-declarative distinction. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00161.
  46. Ortu, D., & Vaidya, M. (2017). The challenges of integrating behavioral and neural data: bridging and breaking boundaries across levels of analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 40, 209–224.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-016-0074-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Otmakhova, N., Duzel, E., Deutch, A. Y., & Lisman, J. (2013). The hippocampal-VTA loop: the role of novelty and motivation in controlling the entry of information into long-term memory. In G. Baldassarre & M. Mirolli (Eds.), Intrinsically motivated learning in natural and artificial systems (pp. 235–254). Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Palmer, D. C. (1991). A behavioral interpretation of memory. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), International Institute on Verbal Relations. Dialogues on verbal behavior: the first International Institute on Verbal Relations (pp. 261–279). Reno, NV: Context Press.Google Scholar
  49. Palmer, D. C. (2009a). Response strength and the concept of the repertoire. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 10, 49–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Palmer, D. C. (2009b). The role of private events in the interpretation of complex behavior. Behavior and Philosophy, 37, 3–19.Google Scholar
  51. Palmer, D. C. (2016). On intraverbal control and the definition of the intraverbal. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 32, 96–106.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-016-0061-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Palmer, D. C., & Donahoe, J. W. (1992). Essentialism and selectionism in cognitive science and behavior analysis. American Psychologist, 47, 1344–1358.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.47.11.1344.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Price, J. L., Ko, A. I., Wade, M. J., Tsou, S. K., McKeel, D. W., & Morris, J. C. (2001). Neuron number in the entorhinal cortex and CA1 in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, 58, 1395–1402.  https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.58.9.1395.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Rankin, C. H., Abrams, T., Barry, R. J., Bhatnagar, S., Clayton, D. F., Colombo, J., … McSweeney, F. K. (2009). Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92, 135–138.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.012.
  55. Redgrave, P., Prescott, T. J., & Gurney, K. (1999). The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? Neuroscience, 89, 1009–1023.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522(98)00319-4.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Ritzer, G. (1975). Sociology: a multiple paradigm science. The American Sociologist, 10, 156–167.Google Scholar
  57. Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803.Google Scholar
  58. Rudy, J. W., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1999). Contextual fear conditioning, conjunctive representations, pattern completion, and the hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113, 867–880.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.113.5.867.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Scatton, B., Simon, H., Le Moal, M., & Bischoff, S. (1980). Origin of dopaminergic innervation of the rat hippocampal formation. Neuroscience Letters, 18, 125–131.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3940(80)90314-6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Shepard, R. N. (1967). Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 156–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Sidman, M. (1958). Some notes on “bursts” in free-operant avoidance experiments. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 167–172.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  62. Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: a research story. Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative.Google Scholar
  63. Skinner, B. F. (1948). The William James lectures. Retrieved from bfskinner.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/02/William-James-Lectures.pdf
  64. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley Publishing Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York, NY: Vintage.Google Scholar
  66. Skinner, B. F. (1977). Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behavior, 5, 1–10.Google Scholar
  67. Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82, 171–177.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Squire, L. R., Wixted, J. T., & Clark, R. E. (2007). Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: a new perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 872–883.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2154.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  69. Staddon, J. E. (2016). Adaptive behavior and learning. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  70. Thompson, R. F., & Spencer, W. A. (1966). Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychological Review, 73, 16–43.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022681.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Wais, P. E., Wixted, J. T., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, L. R. (2006). The hippocampus supports both the recollection and the familiarity components of recognition memory. Neuron, 49, 459–466.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.12.020.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  72. Wang, W. C., Ranganath, C., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2014). Activity reductions in perirhinal cortex predict conceptual priming and familiarity-based recognition. Neuropsychologia, 52, 19–26.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Watkins, M. J. (1990). Mediationism and the obfuscation of memory. American Psychologist, 45, 328–335.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.45.3.328.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Wiggs, C. L., & Martin, A. (1998). Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 227–233.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80144-X.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Wixted, J. (2008). JEAB and the Skinnerian interpretation of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 137–139.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2008.89-137.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  76. Wright, A. A. (2007). An experimental analysis of memory processing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88, 405–433.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.88-405.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  77. Xiang, J.-Z., & Brown, M. W. (1998). Differential neuronal encoding of novelty, familiarity and recency in regions of the anterior temporal lobe. Neuropharmacology, 37, 657–676.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00030-6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: a review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441–517.  https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2002.2864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Yovel, G., & Paller, K. A. (2004). The neural basis of the butcher-on-the-bus phenomenon: when a face seems familiar but is not remembered. NeuroImage, 21, 789–800.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.034.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Neurobehavioral Laboratory, Department of Behavior AnalysisUniversity of North TexasDentonUSA

Personalised recommendations