Abstract
Purpose of Review
We briefly review the literature on behavioral inhibition (BI) in childhood and its associated social and emotional outcomes. We review the interplay of automatic and controlled attention processes in BI children and outline the relations between childhood BI and two components of effortful control (EC): response inhibition and attention switching.
Recent Findings
Contemporary research in cognitive and behavioral neuroscience indicates that components of EC differentially impact developmental risk for BI children. Response inhibition may inflate the risk of anxiety issues by promoting the inefficient deployment of attentional resources in social contexts, while attention shifting may serve as a protective factor by supporting dynamic social information processing.
Summary
The attentional processes subsumed under EC have diverse implications for the developmental trajectory of BI. Further research is necessary to identify the exact mechanisms by which the components of EC affect the manifestation of BI across development, and how this knowledge can guide early intervention efforts.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

References
Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been highlighted as: • Of importance
Fox NA, Henderson HA, Marshall PJ, Nichols KE, Ghera MM. Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005;56:235–62. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141532.
Kagan J, Reznick JS, Clarke C, Snidman N, Garcia-Coll C, Steven J. Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Dev. 1984;55:2212–25. doi:10.2307/1129793.
Rubin KH, Coplan RJ, Bowker JC. Social withdrawal in childhood. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:141–71. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163642.Social.
Ladd GW, Kochenderfer-Ladd B, Eggum ND, Kochel KP, McConnell EM. Characterizing and comparing the friendships of anxious-solitary and unsociable preadolescents. Child Dev. 2011;82:1434–53. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01632.x.
Rudasill KM, Rimm-Kaufman SE. Teacher-child relationship quality: the roles of child temperament and teacher-child interactions. Early Child Res Q. 2009;24:107–20. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.12.003.
Valiente C, Swanson J, Lemery-Chalfant K. Kindergartners’ temperament, classroom engagement, and student-teacher relationship: moderation by effortful control. Soc Dev. 2012;21:558–76.
Karevold E, Ystrom E, Coplan RJ, Sanson AV, Mathiesen KS. A prospective longitudinal study of shyness from infancy to adolescence: stability, age-related changes, and prediction of socio-emotional functioning. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2012;40:1167–77. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9635-6.
Chronis-Tuscano A, Degnan KA, Pine DS, Perez-Edgar K, Henderson HA, Diaz Y, et al. Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48:928–35. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ae09df.
Clauss J, Blackford J. Behavioral inhibition and risk for developing social anxiety disorder: a meta-analytic study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:1–13. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.002.Behavioral.
Degnan KA, Almas AN, Henderson HA, Hane AA, Walker OL, Fox NA. Longitudinal trajectories of social reticence with unfamiliar peers across early childhood. Dev Psychol. 2014;50:2311–23. doi:10.1037/a0037751.
• Eggum-Wilkens ND, Reichenberg RE, Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL. Components of effortful control and their relations to children’s shyness. Int J Behav Dev. 2016;40:544–54. doi:10.1177/0165025415597792. An empirical example of how different components of parent-reported effortful control relate to developmental trajectories of shyness across childhood
Tang A, Van Lieshout RJ, Lahat A, Duku E, Boyle MH, Saigal S, et al. Shyness trajectories across the first four decades predict mental health outcomes. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017; doi:10.1007/s10802-017-0265-x.
Grady JS, Karraker K, Metzger A. Shyness trajectories in slow-to-warm-up infants: relations with child sex and maternal parenting. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2012;33:91–101.
Shell MD, Gazelle H, Faldowski RA. Anxious solitude and the middle school transition: a diathesis x stress model of peer exclusion and victimization trajectories. Dev Psychol. 2014;50:1569–83. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2011.11.002.
Markovic A, Bowker J. Friends also matter: Examining friendship adjustment indices as moderators of anxious withdrawal and trajectories of change in psychological maladjustment. Dev Psychol. Forthcoming 2017.
• Henderson HA, Pine DS, Fox NA. Behavioral inhibition and developmental risk: a dual-processing perspective. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:207–24. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.189. A theoretical model describing the impact of automatic and controlled processing on developmental outcomes of BI children
Davis M. Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of fear conditioning using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Behav Neurosci. 1986;100:814–24. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.100.6.814.
LeDoux JE, Iwata J, Cicchetti P, Reis DJ. Different projections of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral correlates of conditioned fear. J Neurosci. 1988;8:2517–29.
Barker TV, Reeb-Sutherland BC, Fox NA. Individual differences in fear potentiated startle in behaviorally inhibited children. Dev Psychobiol. 2014;56:133–41. doi:10.1002/dev.21096.
Guyer AE, Caouette JD, Lee CC, Ruiz SK. Will they like me? Adolescents’ emotional responses to peer evaluation. Int J Behav Dev. 2014;38:155–63. doi:10.1177/0165025413515627.
Jarcho JM, Davis MM, Shechner T, Degnan KA, Henderson HA, Stoddard J, et al. Early-childhood social reticence predicts brain function in preadolescent youths during distinct forms of peer evaluation. Psychol Sci. 2016;27:821–35. doi:10.1177/0956797616638319.
Pérez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, McDermott JM, Chronis-Tuscano A, Pine DS, Fox NA. Attention biases to threat and behavioral inhibition in early childhood shape adolescent social withdrawal. Emotion. 2010;10:349–57. doi:10.1037/a0018486.
Helfinstein SM, Benson B, Perez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, Detloff A, Pine DS, et al. Striatal responses to negative monetary outcomes differ between temperamentally inhibited and non-inhibited adolescents. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:479–85. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.015.
Guyer AE, Benson B, Choate VR, Bar-Haim Y, Perez-Edgar K, Jarcho JM, et al. Lasting associations between early-childhood temperament and late-adolescent reward-circuitry response to peer feedback. Dev Psychopathol. 2014;26:229–43. doi:10.1017/S0954579413000941.
Guyer AE, Nelson EE, Perez-Edgar K, Hardin MG, Roberson-Nay R, Monk CS, et al. Striatal functional alteration in adolescents characterized by early childhood behavioral inhibition. J Neurosci. 2006;26:6399–405. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0666-06.2006.
Bar-Haim Y, Fox NA, Benson B, Guyer AE, Williams A, Nelson EE, et al. Neural correlates of reward processing in adolescents with a history of inhibited temperament. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:1009–18. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02401.x.
White LK, Degnan KA, Henderson HA, Pérez-Edgar K, Walker OL, Shechner T, et al. Developmental relations among behavioral inhibition, anxiety, and attention biases to threat and positive information. Child Dev. 2017;88:141–55. doi:10.1111/cdev.12696.
Hardee JE, Benson BE, Bar-Haim Y, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Chen G, et al. Patterns of neural connectivity during an attention bias task moderates associations between early childhood temperament and internalizing symptoms in young adulthood. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74:273–9. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.036.
Kokin J, Younger A, Gosselin P, Vaillancourtb T. Biased facial expression interpretation in shy children. Infant Child Dev. 2006;3:3–5. doi:10.1002/icd.
Rothbart M, Derryberry D. Development of individual differences in temperament. In: Lamb ME, Brown AL, editors. Advances in developmental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1981.
Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Damon W, Lerner RM, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol 3: social, emotional, and personality development. New York: Wiley; 2006.
Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P. Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Dev. 2001;72:1394–408.
Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:1–23. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516.
Eisenberg N, Smith CL, Sadovsky A, Spinrad TL. Effortful control: relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. In: Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, editors. Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and application. New York: Guilford Press; 2004.
Kochanska G, Knaack A. Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: antecedents, correlates, and consequences. J Pers. 2003;71:1087–112. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.7106008.
Spinrad TL, Stifter CA, Donelan-McCall N, Turner L. Mothers’ regulation strategies in response to toddlers’ affect: links to later emotion self-regulation. Soc Dev. 2004;13:40–55. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00256.x.
Morales S, Fu X, Pérez-Edgar KE. A developmental neuroscience perspective on affect-biased attention. Accid Anal Prev. 2016;21:26–41. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2016.08.001.
Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. 2007;7(2):336–53.
Eysenck MW, Calvo MG. Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory. Cogn Emot. 1992;6:409–34. doi:10.1080/02699939208409696.
Derryberry D, Reed MA. Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111:225–36. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.225.
Gagne JR, Miller MM, Goldsmith HH. Early-but modest-gender differences in focal aspects of childhood temperament. Pers Individ Dif. 2013;55:95–100. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.02.006.
Simonds J, Rothbart M. The temperament in middle childhood questionnaire (TMCQ): A computerized self-report instrument for ages 7–10. Poster presented at the Occasional Temperament Conference, Athens, GA. 2004.
White LK, McDermott JM, Degnan KA, Henderson HA, Fox NA. Behavioral inhibition and anxiety: the moderating roles of inhibitory control and attention shifting. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39:735–47. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9490-x.
• Wolfe CD, Bell MA. Brain electrical activity of shy and non-shy preschool-aged children during executive function tasks. Infant Child Dev. 2014;23:259–72. doi:10.1002/icd.1858. An empirical example of how shyness relates to cortical activation, in the absence of performance benefits, for shy preschool-aged children
Henderson HA. Electrophysiological correlates of cognitive control and the regulation of shyness in children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2010;35:177–93. doi:10.1080/87565640903526538.
van Veen V, Carter CS. The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:593–602. doi:10.1162/08989290260045837.
• Fu X, Taber-Thomas BC, Perez-Edgar K. Frontolimbic functioning during threat-related attention: relations to early behavioral inhibition and anxiety in children. Biol Psychol. 2017;122:98–109. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.08.010. An fMRI study showing that engagement of the dlPFC when orienting away from threat predicts increased anxiety in BI children, suggesting that executive attention is recruited as a compensatory, but not necessarily effective, strategy in BI children
Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41:49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.
Eisenberg N, Shepard SA, Fabes RA, Murphy BC, Guthrie IK. Shyness and children’s emotionality, regulation, and coping: contemporaneous, longitudinal, and across-context relations. Child Dev. 1998;69:767–90. doi:10.2307/1132203.
Wolfe CD, Zhang J, Kim-Spoon J, Ann BM. A longitudinal perspective on the association between cognition and temperamental shyness. Int J Behav Dev. 2014;38:266–76. doi:10.1177/0165025413516257.
Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Murphy BC. Relations of shyness and low sociability to regulation and emotionality. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;68:505–17. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.505.
Eastwood JD, Smilek D, Merikle PM. Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Percept Psychophys. 2001;63:1004–13. doi:10.3758/BF03194519.
Fox E, Russo R, Bowles R, Dutton K. Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001;130:681–700. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.681.
Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cogn Sci. 2005;9:585–94. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011.
Fox E. Processing emotional facial expressions: the role of anxiety and awareness. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2002;2:52–63. doi:10.1080/02699930143000527.
Lobue V, Pérez-Edgar K. Sensitivity to social and non-social threats in temperamentally shy children at-risk for anxiety. Dev Sci. 2014;17:239–47. doi:10.1111/desc.12110.
Reeck C, Egner T. Emotional task management: neural correlates of switching between affective and non-affective task-sets. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci [Internet]. 2015;10:1045–53. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu153.
Malooly AM, Genet JJ, Siemer M. Individual differences in reappraisal effectiveness: the role of affective flexibility. Emotion. 2013;13:302–13. doi:10.1037/a0029980.
• Marcus O, Stanciu O, MacLeod C, Liebregts H, Visu-Petra L. A FISTful of emotion: individual differences in trait anxiety and cognitive-affective flexibility during preadolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44:1231–42. doi:10.1037/a0029980. An empirical study demonstrating that trait anxiety is associated with deficits in the ability to flexibly engage with and disengage from the affective dimension of human facial stimuli in a matching task
Mocan O, Stanciu O, Visu-Petra L. Relating individual differences in internalizing symptoms to emotional attention set-shifting in children. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2014;27:509–26. doi:10.1080/10615806.2014.888419.
De Lissnyder E, Koster EHW, Derakhshan N, De Raedt R. The association between depressive symptoms and executive control impairments in response to emotional and non-emotional information. Cogn Emot. 2010;24:264–80. doi:10.1080/02699930903378354.
Genet JJ, Malooly AM, Siemer M. Flexibility is not always adaptive: affective flexibility and inflexibility predict rumination use in everyday life. Cogn Emot. 2013;27:1–11. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.733351.
Evans DE, Rothbart MK. Developing a model for adult temperament. J Res Pers. 2007;41:868–88. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.11.002.
Eisenberg N, Eggum ND, Sallquist J, Edwards A. Relations of self-regulatory/control capacities to maladjustment, social competence, and emotionality. In: Hoyle RH, editor. Handbook of personality and self-regulation. New York: Wiley; 2010.
Chu BC, Crocco ST, Esseling P, Areizaga MJ, Lindner AM, Skriner LC. Transdiagnostic group behavioral activation and exposure therapy for youth anxiety and depression: initial randomized controlled trial. Behav Res Ther. 2016;76:65–75. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2015.11.005.
Susa G, Benga O, Pitica I, Miclea M. Child temperamental reactivity and self-regulation effects on attentional biases. Front Psychol. 2014;5(922):1–11. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00922.
Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, et al. Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2004;11:230–41. doi:10.1093/clipsy/bph077.
Flook L, Smalley SL, Kitil MJ, Galla BM, Kaiser-Greenland S, Locke J, et al. Effects of mindful awareness practices on executive functions in elementary school children. J Appl Sch Psychol. 2010;26:70–95. doi:10.1080/15377900903379125.
Napoli M, Krech PR, Holley LC. Mindfulness training for elementary school students: the attention academy. J Appl Sch Psychol. 2005;21:99–125. doi:10.1300/J008v21n01_05.
Mendelson T, Greenberg MT, Dariotis JK, Gould LF, Rhoades BL, Leaf PJ. Feasibility and preliminary outcomes of a school-based mindfulness intervention for urban youth. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38:985–94. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9418-x.
Nadler R, Cordy M, Stengel J, Segal Z V., Hayden EP. A brief mindfulness practice increases self-reported calmness in young children: A pilot study. Mindfulness. 2017;8:1–8. Doi:10.1007/s12671–017–0685-2
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
Dr. Heather Henderson declares grants from Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (Grant number: 435-2016-0494). McLennon Wilson declares that he has no conflicts of interest.
Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent
This article does not contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of the authors.
Additional information
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Child and Developmental Psychiatry
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Henderson, H.A., Wilson, M.J. Attention Processes Underlying Risk and Resilience in Behaviorally Inhibited Children. Curr Behav Neurosci Rep 4, 99–106 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40473-017-0111-z
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40473-017-0111-z
Keywords
- Behavioral inhibition
- Executive attention
- Effortful control
- Response inhibition
- Attention shifting
- Affective flexibility