Abstract
Background
Population ageing is a phenomenon taking place in almost every global region.
Current estimates indicate that 10–20% of older people in developed countries have mild cognitive impairment (MCI), with these percentages predicted to rise markedly by 2050.
Objective
Our objective was to apply eye-tracking technology to investigate the information processes adopted by older people with and without MCI in determining preferences for health states in the five-level EuroQol-5 Dimensions (EQ-5D-5L) instrument.
Methods
Older people (aged ≥ 65 years; including both patients and family carers) attending outpatient memory clinics in Southern Adelaide between July 2017 and June 2018, competent to read and converse in English and with a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) cognition score of ≥ 19 were invited to participate. In total, 52 people met the inclusion criteria, of whom 20 (38%) provided informed consent and fully participated. Participants were categorised into two subgroups (each n = 10) for comparison based upon established MMSE cognition thresholds (19–23, lower MMSE indicative of MCI; ≥ 24, higher MMSE indicative of good cognition). A discrete-choice experiment (DCE) comprising a series of pairwise choices between alternative EQ-5D-5L health states of varying survival duration with differential levels of task complexity (approximated by the degree of attribute level overlap in each choice), was administered as a face-to-face interview with the participant wearing an eye-tracking device.
Results
Attribute non-attendance (ANA) was higher for the lower MMSE subgroup than for the higher MMSE subgroup, although these differences were generally not statistically significant. ANA remained relatively low and consistent for participants with good cognition regardless of task complexity. In contrast, ANA increased notably in participants exhibiting MCI, increasing from 10% on average per participant in the lower MMSE subgroup with five attribute level overlap to 23% on average per participant in the lower MMSE subgroup with zero attribute level overlap.
Conclusions
This exploratory study provided important insights into the information processes adopted by older people with varying levels of cognitive functioning when choosing between alternative EQ-5D-5L health states of varying survival duration and specifically the relationships between cognitive capacity, task complexity and the extent of ANA. Recent advances in econometric modelling of health state valuation data have demonstrated the added value of capturing ANA information as this can be accounted for in the DCE data analysis, thereby improving the precision of model estimates. Eye-tracking technology can usefully inform the design, conduct and econometric modelling of DCEs, driving the inclusion of this rapidly growing population traditionally excluded from preference-elicitation studies of this nature.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Data Availability
The dataset and software code underpinning this research are available upon request from the study authors.
References
Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour M, et al. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective study. BMJ. 2012;344:d7622.
World Health Organisation and Alzheimers Diseases International. Dementia: a public health priority. World Health Organisation. 2012
Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel K. Normal cognitive aging. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013;29(4):737–52.
Soekhai V, de Bekker-Grob E, Ellis A, Vass C. Discrete choice experiments in health economics: past, present and future. Pharmacoeconomics. 2019;37(2):201–26.
Brazier J, Ratcliffe J, Salomon J, Tsuchiya A. Measuring and valuing health benefits for economic evaluation. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2017.
Cairns J, van der Pol M, Lloyd A. Decision making heuristics and the elicitation of preferences: being fast and frugal about the future. Health Econ. 2002;11(7):655–8.
Erdem S, Campblee D, Hole A. Accounting for attribute-level non-attendance in a health choice experiment: does it matter? Health Econ. 2015;24(7):773–89.
Milte R, Ratcliffe J, Chen G, Crotty M. What characteristics of nursing homes are most valued by consumers? A discrete choice experiment with residents and family members. Value Health. 2018;21(7):843–9.
Veldwijk J, Determann D, Lambooij M, van Til JA, Korfage IJ, de Bekker-Grob EW, et al. Exploring how individuals complete the choice tasks in a discrete choice experiment: an interview study. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2016;16:45.
Milte R, Ratcliffe J, Chen G, Lancsar E, Miller MD, Crotty M. Cognitive overload? An exploration of the potential impact of cognitive functioning in discrete choice experiments with older people in health care. Value Health. 2014;17(5):655–9.
Just MA, Carpenter PA. A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. Psychol Rev. 1980;87(4):329–54.
Pereira MLGF, Camargo MVZA, Aprahamian I, Forlenza OV. Eye movement analysis and cognitive processing: detecting indicators of conversion to Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2014;10:1273–85.
Vidal L, Antúnez L, Sapolinski A, Giménez A, Maiche A, Ares G. Can eye-tracking techniques overcome a limitation of conjoint analysis? Case study on healthfulness perception of yogurt labels. J Sens Stud. 2013;28(5):370–80.
Rigby D, Vass C, Payne K. Opening the ‘black box’: an overview of methods to investigate the decision-making process in choice-based surveys. Patient. 2020;13:31–41.
Kahn B, Louviere J, Townsend C, Wise C. Insights into decisions from neuroscience and choice experiments: the effect of eye movements on choice. Adv Consum Res. 2012;40:774.
Ryan M, Krucien N, Hermens F. The eyes have it: using eye tracking to inform information processing strategies in multi-attributes choices. Health Econ. 2018;27(4):709–21.
Spinks J, Mortimer D. Lost in the crowd? Using eye-tracking to investigate the effect of complexity on attribute non-attendance in discrete choice experiments. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2015;16:14.
Milte R, Huynh E, Ratcliffe J. Assessing quality of care in nursing homes using discrete choice experiments: how does the level of cognitive functioning impact upon older people's preferences? Soc Sci Med. 2019;238:112466.
Hole A, Norman R, Viney R. Response patterns in health state valuation using endogenous attribute attendance and latent class analysis. Health Econ. 2016;25(2):212–24.
Norman R, Cronin P, Viney R. A pilot discrete choice experiment to explore preferences for EQ-5D-5L health states. Appl Health Econ Health Policy. 2013;11:287–98.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–98.
Tombaugh TN, McIntyre NJ. The Mini-Mental State Examination: a comprehensive review. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40:922–35.
Tobii. Tobii Pro Glasses 2 2019 [cited 2019 20 March]. https://www.tobiipro.com/product-listing/tobii-pro-glasses-2/.
Uggeldahl K, Jacobsen C, Lundhede T, Olsen S. Choice certainty in discrete choice experiments: will eye tracking provide useful measures? J Choice Model. 2016;20:35–48.
Heidenreich S, Watson V, Ryan M, Phimister E. Decision heuristic or preference? Attribute non-attendance in discrete choice problems. Health Econ. 2018;27(1):157–71.
Tervonen T, Gelhorn H, Sri Bhashyam S, Poon J, Gries K, Rentz A, Marsh K. MCDA swing weighting and discrete choice experiments for elicitation of patient benefit-risk preferences: a critical assessment. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2017;26(12):1483–91.
IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 23.0. Armonk: IBM Corp; 2015.
Jonker M, Donkers B, de Bekker-Grob E, Stolk E. Attribute level overlap (and colour coding) can reduce task complexity, improve choice consistency and decrease the dropout rate in discrete choice experiments. Health Econ. 2019;28:350–63.
Mitchell AJ. A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the mini-mental state examination in the detection of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43(4):411–31.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors conceived and designed the study. JR and CB oversaw the data analysis. KW performed the data analysis. JR led and KW, CB, RN, SG and CW contributed to drafting the article. All authors read and approved the final article.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Funding
This study was funded in part by an advanced studies scholarship awarded by Flinders University to Kaiying Wang.
Conflict of interest
Julie Ratcliffe, Kaiying Wang, Chris Barr, Richard Norman, Stacey George and Craig Whitehead have no conflicts of interest that are directly relevant to the content of this article.
Ethics approval
This study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee (Australia; HREC/17/SAC/58) and was performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Wang, K., Barr, C., Norman, R. et al. Using Eye-Tracking Technology with Older People in Memory Clinics to Investigate the Impact of Mild Cognitive Impairment on Choices for EQ-5D-5L Health States Preferences. Appl Health Econ Health Policy 19, 111–121 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40258-020-00588-3
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40258-020-00588-3