Abstract
Previous work shows that as compared to European Americans, Asians are more holistic (or less focused) in perceptual processing. Drawing on this evidence, we expected that extra attention control would be recruited to perform focused processing for Asians and holistic processing for European Americans. Eight Asian American and 13 European American young adults judged whether a given framed-line was the same in length as the framed-line shown on the previous trial on the basis of either an absolute or relative criterion. Their brain activities were monitored with functional near infrared spectroscopy. As predicted, Asian Americans showed an increased activation in the parietal regions during the absolute (vs. relative) task, whereas European Americans showed an increased activation in the same regions during the relative (vs. absolute) task. No comparable pattern was observed for performance. The current work provides further evidence for compensatory attention that is engaged during culturally non-preferred tasks.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.




Notes
We thank Dr. Laura Ann Petitto for making this software available to us, for details see Kovelman et al. 2009).
Because the data collection of the frontal brain region was unsuccessful from three participants (one Asian American and two European American participants), the data from 18 participants were analyzed for the brain activity in this region.
References
Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2004). Selection, integration, and conflict monitoring; assessing the nature and generality of prefrontal cognitive control mechanisms. Neuron, 41(3), 473–487.
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. L. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6), 925–945.
Boas, D. A., Chen, K., Grebert, D., & Franceschini, M. A. (2004). Improving the diffuse optical imaging spatial resolution of the cerebral hemodynamic response to brain activation in humans. Optical Letters, 29(13), 1506–1508.
Bohmer, A., Mast, F., & Jarchow, T. (1996). Can a unilateral loss of otolithic function be clinically detected by assessment of the subjective visual vertical? Brain Research Bulletin, 40(5–6), 423–427.
Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(35), 12629–12633.
Duffy, S., Toriyama, R., Itakura, S., & Kitayama, S. (2009). The development of culturally-contingent attention strategies in young children in the U.S. and Japan. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 351–359.
Delpy, D. T., Cope, M., van der Zee, P., Arridge, S., Wray, S., & Wyatt, J. (1988). Estimation of optical pathlength through tissue from direct time of flight measurement. Physics in Medicine & Biology, 33(12), 1433–1442. doi:10.1088/0031-9155/33/12/008.
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19(1), 12–17.
Heilman, K. M., & Van Den Abell, T. (1980). Right hemisphere dominance for attention: The mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology, 30(3), 327–330.
Heine, S. J. (2012). Cultural psychology (2nd ed.). New York, NK: WW Norton & Company.
Hoshi, Y., Kobayashi, N., & Tamura, M. (2001). Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: A study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. Journal of Applied Physiology, 90(5), 1657–1662.
Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A., & Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: A review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics, 48(10), D280–D298.
Imada, T., Carlson, S. M., & Itakura, S. (2013). East-West cultural differences in context-sensitivity are evident in early childhood. Developmental Science, 16(2), 198–208. doi:10.1111/desc.12016.
Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (2007). Handbook of cultural psychology. New York: Guilford Press.
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T., & Larsen, J. T. (2003). Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: A cultural look at new look. Psychological Science, 14(3), 201–206.
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(6), 1245–1267.
Kitayama, S., & Murata, A. (2013). Culture modulates perceptual attention: An event-related potential study. Social Cognition, 31(6), 758–769.
Kitayama, S., & Park, J. (2014). Error-related brain activity reveals self-centric motivation: Culture matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 62–70. doi:10.1037/a0031696.
Kitayama, S., Park, H., Servincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K. (2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Comparing North America, West Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(2), 236–255.
Kitayama, S., & Uskul, A. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 419–449.
Kovelman, I., Shalinsky, M. H., Berens, M. S., & Petitto, L. A. (2014). Words in the bilingual brain: An fNIRS brain imaging investigation of lexical processing in sign-speech bimodal bilinguals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 606.
Kovelman, I., Shalinsky, M. H., White, K. S., Schmitt, S. N., Berens, M. S., Paymer, N., & Petitto, L. (2009). Dual language use in sign-speech bimodal bilinguals: fNIRS brain-imaging evidence. Brain and Language, 109(2–3), 112–123.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 922–934.
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2006). Culture and change blindness. Cognitive Science, 30, 381–399.
Na, J., & Kitayama, S. (2011). Trait-based person perception is culture-specific: Behavioral and neural evidence. Psychological Science, 22(8), 1025–1032.
Na, J., & Kitayama, S. (2012). Will people work hard on a task they choose? Both culture and social-eyes priming matter. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 284–290.
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291–310.
Okamoto, M., Dan, H., Sakamoto, K., Takeo, K., Shimizu, K., Kohno, S., Oda, I., Isobe, S., Suzuki, T., Kohyama, K., & Dan, I. (2004). Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10-20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping. Neuroimage, 21(1), 99–111.
Riccio, G. E., Martin, E. J., & Stoffregen, T. A. (1992). The role of balance dynamics in the active perception of orientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 624–644.
Shalinsky, M. H., Kovelman, I., Berens, M. S., & Petitto, L. A. (2009). Exploring cognitive functions in babies, children & adults with near infrared spectroscopy. Journal of Visualized Experiments,. doi:10.3791/1268.
Shulman, G. L., Pope, D. L., Astafiev, S. V., McAvoy, M. P., Snyder, A. Z., & Corbetta, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dominance during spatial selective attention and target detection occurs outside the dorsal frontoparietal network. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(10), 3640–3651.
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283, 1657–1661.
Varnum, M. E. W., Grossmann, I., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). The origin of cultural differences in cognition: The social orientation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 9–13.
Villringer, A., & Chance, B. (1997). Non-invasive optical spectroscopy and imaging of human brain function. Trends in Neurosciences, 20(10), 435–442.
Wager, T. D., & Smith, E. E. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of working memory: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(4), 255–274.
Acknowledgments
We thank Dr. Petitto for affording us with the fNIRS data analyses software. We also thank fNIRS laboratory at the Center for Human Growth and Development (CHGD) and CHGD of the University of Michigan.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding authors
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Murata, A., Park, J., Kovelman, I. et al. Culturally non-preferred cognitive tasks require compensatory attention: a functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) investigation. Cult. Brain 3, 53–67 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-015-0027-y
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-015-0027-y
Keywords
- Culture
- Framed-line task
- Holistic vs. analytic attention
- fNIRS