Advertisement

Biological Theory

, Volume 12, Issue 4, pp 195–210 | Cite as

The Social Trackways Theory of the Evolution of Language

  • Kim Shaw-WilliamsEmail author
Thematic Issue Article: Symbols, Signals, and the Archaeological Record II

Abstract

The social trackways theory (for full introduction see my previous article on the topic in Biological Theory) is centered on the remarkable 3.66 mya Laetoli Fossilized Trackways, for they incontrovertibly reveal our ancestors were already obligate bipeds with very human-like feet, and were intentionally stepping in other band members’ footprints to maintain safe footing. Trackways are unique among natural sign systems in possessing a depictive narratively generative structure, somewhat like the symbolic sign systems of gestural languages. Therefore, due to daily embodied reiteration of their own and other band member’s old footprints, both for bipedal safety and as recognizable wayfinding markers for socio-ecological navigation, incrementally our Mid-Pliocene ancestors began to acquire a cognitive capacity for episodic personal memories and episodic future simulations. They began mentally representing themselves (and others) as intentional agents continuously travelling from the past into the future. This spatially and temporally self-projecting awareness manifested itself as extra theory of mind and mental space-and-time-travel capacities, which increasingly enabled intentional or conscious, top-down executive adjustment of past behaviors for the sake of achieving better ways of doing things in the future. Future-directed behavioral and cultural adaptations began to increase fitness in all domains, thus creating powerful selective feedback for further entrenchment. Here we focus on overtly or consciously intentional exploratory wayfinding and transmission of elsewhere-and-when information, using pragmatic, discursive modes of communication, which enabled far more cooperative and efficient foraging practices. Selection for better communication thus led to intentional trail marking and the earliest emergence of more conventional/symbolic depictive and gestural “protolanguages.”

Keywords

Agential self Evolution of language Extra theory of mind Mental space and time travel Observational self Pragmatic discourse 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Earlier drafts of this article were written while I was fully supported by a PhD Research Scholarship from the Australian National University. Many thanks to my present and earlier supervisors Kim Sterelny, Peter Hiscock, Ben Jeffares, Brett Calcott, and Russell Gray; my past and present fellow students, especially Adrian Currie and Anton Killin; my walking/sounding-board friend Antek Skotnicki, and my computer-guru friend John Mills in Lyneham, Canberra; my oldest/longest-suffering nonacademic friends—and, of course, my family—for, let’s face it, their extreme patience, combined with discerning yet always encouraging critiques and/or pragmatic/emotional support. I am also very grateful to friends Ben Frazer and Kirsty Douglas for harboring my old camper van on their property whenever I am not using it as temporary accommodation in Canberra.

References

  1. Alemseged Z, Spoor F, Kimbel WH et al (2006) A juvenile early hominin skeleton from Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature 443(7109):296–301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andrews-Hanna JR (2012) The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist 18(3):251–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Attwell L, Kovarovic K, Kendal JR (2015) Fire in the Plio-Pleistocene: the functions of hominin fire use, and the mechanistic, developmental and evolutionary consequences. J Anthropol Sci 93:1–20Google Scholar
  4. Barboni D (2014) Vegetation of Northern Tanzania during the Plio-Pleistocene: a synthesis of the paleobotanical evidences from Laetoli, Olduvai, and Peninj hominin sites. Quatern Int 322–323:264–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bekoff M (2001) Observations of scent-marking and discriminating self from others by a domestic dog (Canis familiaris): tales of displaced yellow snow. Behav Process 55(2):75–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bennett MR, Reynolds SC, Morse SA, Budka M (2016) Laetoli’s lost tracks: 3D generated mean shape and missing footprints. Sci Rep 6:21916CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bobe R, Alemseged Z, Behrensmeyer AK (2007) Approaches to the analysis of faunal change during the East African Pliocene. In: Bobe R, Alemseged Z, Behrensmeyer AK (eds) Hominin environments in the East African Pliocene: an assessment of the faunal evidence. Springer, Berlin, pp 1–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Boyd B (2017) The evolution of stories: from mimesis to language, from fact to fiction. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, HobokenGoogle Scholar
  9. Briana B (2014) Male Tibetan macaques′ (Macaca thibetana) choice of infant bridging partners. Zool Res 35(3):222Google Scholar
  10. Brumm A, Van Den Bergh GD, Storey M et al (2016) Age and context of the oldest known hominin fossils from Flores. Nature 534(7606):249–253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Burkart JM, Hrdy SB, Schaik CPV. (2009) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolut Anthropol 18(5):175–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Claessens L, Veldkamp A, Schoorl JM et al (2016) Large scale pantelleritic ash flow eruptions during the Late Miocene in central Kenya and evidence for significant environmental impact. Global Planet Change 145:30–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Corballis MC (2014) The recursive mind: the origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton University Press, PrincetonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Corballis MC (2017) Language evolution: a changing perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, AmsterdamCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Crompton RH, Pataky TC, Savage R et al (2012) Human-like external function of the foot, and fully upright gait, confirmed in the 3.66 million year old Laetoli hominin footprints by topographic statistics, experimental footprint-formation and computer simulation. J R Soc Interface 9(69):707–719.  http://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2011.0258 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Currie A, Sterelny K (2017) In defence of story-telling. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  17. de la Torre I (2016) The origins of the Acheulean: past and present perspectives on a major transition in human evolution. Phil Trans R Soc B 371(1698):20150245CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Diogo R, Molnar JL, Wood B (2017) Bonobo anatomy reveals stasis and mosaicism in chimpanzee evolution, and supports bonobos as the most appropriate extant model for the common ancestor of chimpanzees and humans. Sci Rep 7(1):608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ferretti F (2014) Travelling in time and space at the origins of language. Hum Mente J Philos Stud 27:243–268Google Scholar
  20. Ferretti F, Adornetti I, Chiera A et al (2017) Mental Time Travel and language evolution: a narrative account of the origins of human communication. Language Sciences, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  21. Gatty H (1998) Finding your way without map or compass. Courier Corporation, Dover Edition, MineolaGoogle Scholar
  22. Green DJ, Alemseged Z (2012) Australopithecus afarensis scapular ontogeny, function, and the role of climbing in human evolution. Science 338(6106):514–517.  http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1227123 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Haile-Selassie Y, Latimer BM, Alene M et al (2010) An early Australopithecus afarensis postcranium from Woranso-Mille, Ethiopia. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107(27):12121–12126.  http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1004527107 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Haile-Selassie Y, Latimer B, Lovejoy CO et al (2016) Conclusion: implications of KSD-VP-1/1 for early hominin paleobiology and insights into the chimpanzee/human last common ancestor. In: Haile-Selassie Y, Su DF (eds), The Postcranial Anatomy of Australopithecus afarensis. Springer, Netherlands, pp 179–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Harmand S, Lewis JE, Feibel CS et al (2015) 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature 521(7552):310–315.  http://doi.org/10.1038/nature14464 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hlubik S, Berna F, Feibel C, Braun D, Harris JW (2017) Researching the nature of fire at 1.5 mya on the site of FxJj20 AB, Koobi Fora, Kenya, using high-resolution spatial analysis and FTIR spectrometry. Curr Anthropol 58(S16):S000–S000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hublin JJ, Ben-Ncer A, Bailey SE et al (2017) New fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco and the pan-African origin of Homo sapiens. Nature 546(7657):289–292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jeffares B (2014) Back to australopithecus: utilizing new theories of cognition to understand the Pliocene hominins. Biol Theory 9(1):4–15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Joordens JCA, Kuipers RS, Wanink JH, Muskiet FAJ (2014) A fish is not a fish: patterns in fatty acid composition of aquatic food may have had implications for hominin evolution. J Hum Evolut 77:107–116.  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.04.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Killin A (2016) Musicality and the evolution of mind, mimesis, and entrainment. Biol Philos 31(3):421–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kissel M, Hawks J (2015) What are the Lothagam and Tabarin Mandibles? PaleoAnthropology 37:43Google Scholar
  32. Kuhn SL, Raichlen DA, Clark AE (2016) What moves us? How mobility and movement are at the center of human evolution. Evolut Anthropol 25(3):86–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Laden G, Wrangham R (2005) The rise of the hominids as an adaptive shift in fallback foods: plant underground storage organs (USOs) and australopith origins. J Human Evol 49(4):482–498CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Leakey MD (1981) Discoveries at Laetoli in northern Tanzania. Proc Geologists’ Assoc, 92(2), 81–86.  http://doi.org/10.1016/s0016-7878(81)80008-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Leakey MD, Harris JM (1987) Laetoli, a Pliocene site in northern Tanzania. Clarendon Press. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  36. Leakey MG, Spoor F, Brown FH et al (2001) New hominin genus from eastern Africa shows diverse middle Pliocene lineages. Nature 410(6827):433–440. http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v410/n6827/suppinfo/410433a0_S1.html
  37. Leakey MG, Spoor F, Dean MC et al (2012) New fossils from Koobi Fora in Northern Kenya confirm taxonomic diversity in early Homo. Nature 488(7410):201–204. http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v488/n7410/abs/nature11322.html#supplementary-information
  38. Lee RB, DeVore I (eds) (1968) Problems in the study of hunters and gatherers. In: Man the Hunter. Aldine Publishing Company, Chicago, pp 3–12Google Scholar
  39. Lieberman DE (2001) Another face in our family tree. Nature 410(6827):419–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lordkipanidze D, Vekua A, Ferring R et al (2005) Anthropology: the earliest toothless hominin skull. Nature 434(7034):717–718CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lovejoy CO, Simpson SW, White TD et al (2009a) Careful climbing in the Miocene: the forelimbs of Ardipithecus ramidus and humans are primitive. Science 326(5949):70, e71–e78.  http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1175827 Google Scholar
  42. Lovejoy CO, Suwa G, Simpson SW et al (2009b) The great divides: Ardipithecus ramidus reveals the postcrania of our last common ancestors with African apes. Science 73(5949):100–106.  http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1175833 Google Scholar
  43. Macho GA (2014) An ecological and behavioural approach to hominin evolution during the Pliocene. Quatern Sci Rev 96:23–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Maestripieri D (1998) The evolution of male-infant interactions in the tribe Papionini (Primates: Cercopithecidae). Folia Primatol 69(5):247–251CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. McPherron SP, Alemseged Z, Marean et al (2010) Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature 466(7308):857–860CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. McPherron SP, Alemseged Z, Marean C et al (2011) Tool-marked bones from before the Oldowan change the paradigm. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108(21):E116.  http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1101298108 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Moore R (2016) Meaning and ostension in great ape gestural communication. Anim Cogn 19(1):223–231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. O’Regan H, Turner A, Bishop L, Elton S, Lamb A (2011) Hominins without fellow travellers? First appearances and inferred dispersals of Afro-Eurasian large-mammals in the Plio-Pleistocene. Quatern Sci Rev 30(11):1343–1352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Organista E, Domínguez-Rodrigo M, Yravedra J et al (2017) Biotic and abiotic processes affecting the formation of BK Level 4c (Bed II, Olduvai Gorge) and their bearing on hominin behavior at the site. Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  50. Osvath M, Gärdenfors P (2007) What are the evolutionary causes of mental time travel? Behav Brain Sci 30(03):329–330.  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07002154 doiCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Palombo MR (2013) What about causal mechanisms promoting early hominin dispersal in Eurasia? A research agenda for answering a hotly debated question. Quatern Int 295:13–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Pataky TC, Mu T, Bosch K et al (2012) Gait recognition: highly unique dynamic plantar pressure patterns among 104 individuals. J R Soc Interface 9(69):790–800CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Paul L (2017) First personal modes of presentation and the structure of empathy. Inquiry 60(3):189–207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Plummer T (2004) Flaked stones and old bones: biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. Am J Phys Anthropol 125(S39):118–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Price SA, Hopkins SSB, Smith KK, Roth VL (2012) Tempo of trophic evolution and its impact on mammalian diversification. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109(18):7008–7012.  http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1117133109 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rolland J, Condamine FL, Beeravolu CR et al (2015) Dispersal is a major driver of the latitudinal diversity gradient of Carnivora. Global Ecol Biogeogr 24(9):1059–1071CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Russon AE, Compost A, Kuncoro P, Ferisa A (2014) Orangutan fish eating, primate aquatic fauna eating, and their implications for the origins of ancestral hominin fish eating. J Human Evol 77:50–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Sayers K, Lovejoy CO, Emery NJ et al (2008) The chimpanzee has no clothes: a critical examination of Pan troglodytes in models of human evolution. Curr Anthropol 49(1):87–114Google Scholar
  59. Sayers K, Raghanti MA, Lovejoy CO (2012) Human evolution and the chimpanzee referential doctrine. Annu Rev Anthropol 41:119–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Scott-Phillips T (2014) Speaking our minds: why human communication is different, and how language evolved to make it special. Palgrave MacMillan, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  61. Shaw-Williams K (2011) The triggering track-ways theory. MA thesis, http://researcharchive.vuw.ac.nz
  62. Shaw-Williams K (2014) The social trackways theory of the evolution of human cognition. Biol Theory 9(1):16–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Shaw-Williams K (forthcoming) The social trackways theory of the evolution of human cognition, culture and communication. Dissertation, Australian National UniversityGoogle Scholar
  64. Silberbauer GB (1981) Hunter and habitat in the central Kalahari Desert. CUP Archive, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  65. Small MF (1990) Alloparental behaviour in Barbary macaques, Macaca sylvanus. Anim Behav 39(2):297–306.  http://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80874-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Sterelny K (2003) Thought in a hostile world: the evolution of human cognition. Blackwell Publishing, MaldenGoogle Scholar
  67. Stewart KM (2014) Environmental change and hominin exploitation of C4-based resources in wetland/savanna mosaics. J Human Evol.  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.10.003 Google Scholar
  68. Suddendorf T (2013) The gap: the science of what separates us from other animals. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  69. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC (2007) The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci 30(3):299–313Google Scholar
  70. Sugiyama MS (2001) Food, foragers, and folklore: the role of narrative in human subsistence. Evolut Human Behav 22:221–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Tomasello M (2009) Constructing a language. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  72. Tomasello M (2014) A natural history of human thinking. Harvard University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Tudryn A, Abdessadok S, Gargani J et al (2016) Stratigraphy and paleoenvironment during the Late Pliocene at Masol paleonto-archeological site (Siwalik Range, NW India): preliminary results. CR Palevol 15(3):440–452CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. ​Tulving E (2005) Episodic memory and autonoesis: uniquely human? In: Terrace HS, Metcalfe J (eds) The missing link in cognition: origins of self-reflective consciousness. Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  75. Van Den Bergh GD, Kaifu Y, Kurniawan I et al (2016) Homo floresiensis-like fossils from the early Middle Pleistocene of Flores. Nature 534(7606):245–248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Villmoare B, Kimbel WH, Seyoum C et al (2015) Early Homo at 2.8 Ma from Ledi-Geraru, Afar, Ethiopia. Science 347(6228):1352–1355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. White TD, Lovejoy CO et al (2015) Neither chimpanzee nor human, Ardipithecus reveals the surprising ancestry of both. Proc Natl Acad Sci 112(16):4877–4884.  http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1403659111 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. WoldeGabriel G, Heiken G, White TD et al (2000) Volcanism, tectonism, sedimentation, and the paleoanthropological record in the Ethiopian Rift System. Geological Society of America, Boulder, pp 83–99Google Scholar
  79. WoldeGabriel G, Ambrose SH, Barboni D (2009) The geological, isotopic, botanical, invertebrate, and lower vertebrate surroundings of Ardipithecus ramidus. Science 65(5949):65e61-65e65.  http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1175817 Google Scholar
  80. Wrangham R, Cheney D, Seyfarth R, Sarmiento E (2009) Shallow-water habitats as sources of fallback foods for hominins. Am J Phys Anthropol 140(4):630–642.  http://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.21122 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Research School of Social Sciences, School of PhilosophyAustralian National UniversityCanberraAustralia

Personalised recommendations