The Evolutionary Rationale for Consciousness

Abstract

To answer the question of why we have consciousness, I propose the following evolutionary trajectory leading to this feature: Nervous systems appeared for the purpose of orchestrating behavior. As a rule of thumb the challenges facing an animal concern either approach or avoidance. These two options were originally hard-wired as reflexes. Improvements in adaptability of response came with an expansion of the computational aspect of the system and a concomitant shift from simple reflexes to instinctual behavior, learning, and eventually, feelings. The assessment of positive and negative feelings allows organisms to weigh various options, but for this to be a viable strategy, an awareness of hedonic value is required. This was presumably the first neural attribute to evolve that required awareness, and thus the key force in the evolution of consciousness. The attribute first appeared in the early amniotes (the phylogenetic group comprising reptiles, birds and mammals). Support for this model in current accounts of the neurobiology of feelings and consciousness is discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Alkire MT, Miller J (2005) General anesthesia and the neural correlates of consciousness. Prog Brain Res 150:229–244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Alkire MT, Hudetz AG, Tononi G (2008) Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 322:876–880

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Baars B (1997) In the theather of consciousness. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  4. Barbano MF, Cador M (2007) Opioids for hedonic experience and dopamine to get ready for it. Psychopharm 191:497–506

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Baumeister RF, Masicampo EJ, Vohs KD (2011) Do conscious thoughts cause behavior? Ann Rev Psych 62:331–361

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML (2011) Building a neuroscience of pleasure and well-being. Psych Well-Being 1:3

    Google Scholar 

  7. Beshkar M (2008) The presence of consciousness in the absence of the cerebral cortex. Synapse 62:553–556

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bittar RG, Kar-Purkayastha I, Owen SL, Bear RE, Green A, Wang S, Aziz TZ (2005) Deep brain stimulation for pain relief: a meta-analysis. J Clin Neurosci 12:515–519

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Borrelli L, Fiorito G (2008) Behavioral analysis of learning and memory in cephalopods. In: Menzel R (ed) Learning theory and behavior. Elsevier, New York, pp 605–628

    Google Scholar 

  10. Butler AB (2008) Evolution of brains, cognition, and consciousness. Brain Res Bul 75:442–449

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Butler AB, Cotterill RM (2006) Mammalian and avian neuroanatomy and the question of consciousness in birds. Biol Bul 211:106–127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Buzsaki G (2007) The structure of consciousness. Nature 446:267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Cabanac M (1992) Pleasure: the common currency. J Theor Biol 155:173–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Cabanac M, Bonniot-Cabanac MC (2007) Decision making: rational or hedonic? Beh Brain Funct 3:45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Cabanac M, Cabanac AJ, Parent A (2009) The emergence of consciousness in phylogeny. Beh Brain Res 198:267–272

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Crick F, Koch C (2003) A framework for consciousness. Nat Neurosci 6:119–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Crick F, Koch C (2005) What is the function of the claustrum? Phil Trans Royal Soc London B 360:1271–1279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Damasio A (1999) The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Harcourt, New York

    Google Scholar 

  19. Denton DA, McKinley MJ, Farrell M, Egan GF (2009) The role of primordial emotions in the evolutionary origin of consciousness. Conscious Cogn 18:500–514

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Edelman G (2004) Wider than the sky: the phenomenal gift of consciousness. Yale University Press, Yale

    Google Scholar 

  21. Edelman DB, Seth AK (2009) Animal consciousness: a synthetic approach. Trends Neurosc 32:476–484

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Ferrarelli F, Tononi G (2011) The thalamic reticular nucleus and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bul 37:306–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Fiorito G, Scotto P (1992) Observational learning in octopus vulgaris. Science 256:545–547

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Gould JL, Grant-Gould C (1995) The honey bee. Scientific American Library, New York

    Google Scholar 

  25. Grinde B (2012) The biology of happiness. Springer, Dordrecht

    Google Scholar 

  26. Gvilia I (2010) Underlying brain mechanisms that regulate sleep-wakefulness cycles. Int Rev Neurobio 93:1–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Hochner B, Shomrat T, Fiorito G (2006) The octopus: a model for a comparative analysis of the evolution of learning and memory mechanisms. Biol Bul 210:308–317

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Jones EG (2001) The thalamic matrix and thalamocortical synchrony. Trends Neurosc 24:595–601

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Kitchen A, Denton D, Brent L (1996) Self-recognition and abstraction abilities in the common chimpanzee studied with distorting mirrors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:7405–7408

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC (2009) Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends Cogn Sci 13:479–487

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Kunde W, Kiesel A, Hoffmann J (2003) Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition 88:223–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Laland KN, Galef BG (2009) The question of animal culture. Harvard University Press, Harvard

    Google Scholar 

  33. Land M, Nilsson D-E (2002) Animal eyes. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  34. Lane N (2009) Life ascending: the ten great inventions of evolution. WW Norton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  35. Leknes S, Tracey I (2008) A common neurobiology for pain and pleasure. Nature Rev Neurosc 9:314–320

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Lieberman MD, Eisenberger NI (2009) Neuroscience pains and pleasures of social life. Science 323:890–891

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Lou HC (2011) Paradigm shift in consciousness research: the child’s self-awareness and abnormalities in autism, ADHD and schizophrenia. Acta Paediat 101:112–119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Marino L (2007) Cetacean brains: how aquatic are they? Anat Rec 290:694–700

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Mather JA (2008) Cephalopod consciousness: behavioural evidence. Conscious Cogn 17:37–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. McFarland DJ, Sibly RM (1975) The behavioural final common path. Philos Trans Royal Soc London B 270:265–293

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Merker B (2007) Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: a challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Beh Brain Sci 30:63–81

    Google Scholar 

  42. Miller JW, Ferrendelli JA (1990) Characterization of GABAergic seizure regulation in the midline thalamus. Neuropharma 29:649–655

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Mink JW, Blumenschine RJ, Adams DB (1981) Ratio of central nervous system to body metabolism in vertebrates: its constancy and functional basis. Am J Phys 241:R203–R212

    Google Scholar 

  44. Moriyama T, Gunji YP (1997) Autonomous learning in maze solution by octopus. Ethology 103:499–513

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Mosley C (2011) Pain and nociception in reptiles. Vet Clin N Am Exotic Ani Prac 14:45–60

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Nir Y, Tononi G (2010) Dreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends Cogn Sci 14:88–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Noirhomme Q, Soddu A, Lehembre R, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Boveroux P, Boly M, Laureys S (2010) Brain connectivity in pathological and pharmacological coma. Front Syst Neurosci 4:160

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Palmiter RD (2011) Dopamine signaling as a neural correlate of consciousness. Neurosci 198:213–220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Panksepp J (1998) Affective neuroscience. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  50. Panksepp J, Normansell L, Cox JF, Siviy SM (1994) Effects of neonatal decortication on the social play of juvenile rats. Phys Behav 56:429–443

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Pecina S (2008) Opioid reward “liking” and “wanting” in the nucleus accumbens. Phys Behav 94:675–680

    Google Scholar 

  52. Pessiglione M, Petrovic P, Daunizeau J, Palminteri S, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (2008) Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron 59:561–567

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Prior H, Schwarz A, Gunturkun O (2008) Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): evidence of self-recognition. PLoS Biol 6:e202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Reiss D, Marino L (2001) Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:5937–5942

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Rose JD (2007) Anthropomorphism and “mental welfare” of fishes. Dis Aqua Org 75:139–154

    Google Scholar 

  56. Rosenthal DM (2008) Consciousness and its function. Neuropsychologia 46:829–840

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Rossano MJ (2003) Expertise and the evolution of consciousness. Cognition 89:207–236

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Schiff ND, Fins JJ (2007) Deep brain stimulation and cognition: moving from animal to patient. Cur Opin Neurol 20:638–642

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Seth AK, Baars BJ, Edelman DB (2005) Criteria for consciousness in humans and other mammals. Conscious Cogn 14:119–139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Smith CU (2010) Darwin’s unsolved problem: the place of consciousness in an evolutionary world. J Hist Neurosci 19:105–120

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Smith K, Mahler S, Pecina S, Berridge K (2010) Hedonic hotspots: generating sensory pleasure in the brain. In: Kringelbach M, Berridge K (eds) Pleasures in the brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 27–49

    Google Scholar 

  62. Sneddon LU (2009) Pain perception in fish: indicators and endpoints. ILAR J 50:338–342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Szymusiak R, McGinty D (2008) Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and arousal. Ann New York Acad Sci 1129:275–286

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Tabibnia G, Satpute AB, Lieberman MD (2008) The sunny side of fairness—preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychol Sci 19:339–347

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Takahashi H, Kato M, Matsuura M, Mobbs D, Suhara T, Okubo Y (2009) When your gain is my pain and your pain is my gain: neural correlates of envy and schadenfreude. Science 323:937–939

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Tamietto M, de Gelder B (2010) Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:697–709

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Ward LM (2011) The thalamic dynamic core theory of conscious experience. Conscious Cogn 20:464–486

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Zhang Y, Lu H, Bargmann CI (2005) Pathogenic bacteria induce aversive olfactory learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 438:179–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Michel Cabanac (Laval University, Quebec, Canada) and David B. Edelman (The Neurosciences Institute, San Diego, CA, USA) for critical reading of the manuscript and for numerous highly valuable comments.

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bjørn Grinde.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Grinde, B. The Evolutionary Rationale for Consciousness. Biol Theory 7, 227–236 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-012-0061-3

Download citation

Keywords

  • Amniotes
  • Consciousness
  • Emotions
  • Evolution
  • Mood modules
  • Self-awareness