Using a combination of behavioural observation and experimental manipulation, we have demonstrated that honey bees remove caps from grapevine flowers, which can lead to a higher pollen yield, and that, depending on the variety, this can reduce the number of caps that stuck to the developing ovaries.
This is the first time cap removal has been demonstrated as a foraging strategy for honey bees. Other bee species have been observed to remove the operculum form Eucalyptus flowers (Houston 2000), but these observations do not automatically lead to the prediction that honey bees behave in the same way. This is because foraging strategies differ between bee species. For example, the use of vibration to collect pollen from buzz pollinated plants (Thorp 2000) and nectar robbing (Maloof and Inouye 2000), have been observed in numerous bee species, but have never been recorded for honey bees. So far, removal of caps from grapevine flowers has not been described for any other bee species.
During the visits of honey bees to grapevine inflorescences, caps fell at a significantly higher rate than at other times. Some of these falling caps would have been hanging in the bunch (‘bunch trash’). However, our experimental finding that honey bee presence reduced the number of stuck-on caps in Merlot and Cabernet Sauvignon demonstrates that at least a proportion of the calyptrae were removed directly off the flowers.
While cap removal by honey bees was demonstrated for Merlot and Cabernet Sauvignon, it was not shown for Chardonnay. This result may be a consequence of our experimental set up: If dried caps are shed during berry growth, we may have been too late to assess effects as, at the time of our experiment, flowering was more advanced in this variety. Alternatively, the Chardonnay flowers may not have been attractive to bees, or the caps may have been on either too tight or too loose for the bees to have an effect on cap removal. Ad hoc observations of all varieties in the vineyard indicated large differences in the number of bees visiting the different flowering varieties (Hogendoorn pers. obs.), but the reasons for this variation visitation are not as yet understood.
It is likely that, by removing caps, the bees increase their pollen yield compared to collection of pollen from flowers that had recently dropped their caps. This is supported by our evidence that manual removal of lose caps lead to a 72 % increase in pollen yield compared to collecting pollen from recently opened flowers. However, the benefit of manual cap removal may not accurately reflect that of cap removal by the bees, as different factors could further influence the pollen yield. For example, bees may be more efficient than humans at capturing the pollen that is released during cap removal as both the hairs on the bee’s body and differences in electric loading between the pollen and the bee will enhance pollen capture (Corbet et al. 1982). In conjunction with this, the air movement produced by the wing beat and body movement of the bee on the inflorescence could cause a reduction of the amount of pollen loss in recently opened flowers. On the other hand, the movement of the bee during uncapping may cause more pollen to be distributed by the wind than our careful manual cap removal. Thus, while the existence of benefits for cap removal in terms of pollen collecting rate seems likely, this remains to be quantified and such quantification will require incorporation of search and handling times.
The bees preferentially visited the more rewarding inflorescences, i.e. inflorescences with high rates of capfall, but how did they recognise them? Visual recognition seems unlikely, as flowers of grapevine are small and green which makes them visually unattractive to bees, and they are often situated under foliage, where it is relatively dark. Because the bees distinguished between more and less rewarding inflorescences without landing, it seems likely that they identified the stage of the inflorescences using volatiles. Grapevine does not produce nectar, but there are several other possible sources of volatiles. Firstly, it has been suggested that stigmatic exudate, only present in recently uncapped flowers, is attractive for bees (Vasconcelos et al. 2009). Secondly, the pollen itself produces volatiles that are attractive to insects (Martin et al. 2009). Thirdly, it is possible that the bees specifically recognised the grape flowers with loose caps, for example on the basis of sesquiterpenes, that are released under the cap in the period just before capfall (Martin et al. 2009) and would be discharged when the cap first opens under pressure. Sesquiterpenes play multiple ecological roles, e.g. in chemical communication in bees (Leonhardt 2010), in defence against pathogens and in pollinator attraction (Huang et al. 2012). The ability of bees to directly detect grape flowers with loose caps and the possible role of volatiles in this recognition need to be verified.
While pollination by bees is unimportant in nearly all grape varieties (Free 1993; Vasconcelos et al. 2009), the removal of caps by honey bees could be important in several ways. For example, caps that fall from the flowers but are retained in the bunches can be sources of Botrytis infections later in the growing season (Nair et al. 1988). In addition, cap removal by honey bees could be important for varieties where persistence of the calyptra has consequences for fertilisation and consequent development of the fruit. One such variety is Pinot Noir, where the occurrence of millerandage has been associated with failure of caps to fall (Friend et al. 2003), and persistence of the calyptra causes the formation of live green ovaries (Heazlewood and Wilson 2004). Other varieties that potentially suffer from this phenomenon are Merlot and Shiraz. However, in these varieties, the importance of cap retention for the development of millerandage has not as yet been established.
Apart from a possibility to deploy honey bees for the reduction of millerandage, bees could also be used for targeted delivery of biological control agents for control of Botrytis bunch rot. This disease, caused by the fungus Botrytis cinerea, is responsible for significant economic damage in vineyards worldwide (Elmer and Michailides 2007; La Guerche et al. 2006). The pathogen enters the floral tissue, either through the cap scar or through the style and then remains latent, resuming pathogenic development as the fruit ripens (e.g. Holz et al. 2003). To prevent latent infection, two or three fungicidal sprays against B. cinerea are recommended during flowering. However, due to both the growing incidence of resistance against synthetic fungicides and concerns for environmental health, the industry is moving increasingly towards the use of biological control agents (Elmer and Reglinski 2006). A beneficial Trichoderma species, Trichoderma koningii, is currently marketed in Australia for the control of latent infection of Botrytis bunch rot in grapevine (Metcalf 2012). The foraging preferences of the honey bees would place them at the right location and time to deliver biological control agents for control of Botrytis bunch rot through entomovectoring (Mommaerts and Smagghe 2011). Trials for this application are currently under way.