Abstract
After introducing the new field of cultural evolution, we review a growing body of empirical evidence suggesting that culture shapes what people attend to, perceive and remember as well as how they think, feel and reason. Focusing on perception, spatial navigation, mentalizing, thinking styles, reasoning (epistemic norms) and language, we discuss not only important variation in these domains, but emphasize that most researchers (including philosophers) and research participants are psychologically peculiar within a global and historical context. This rising tide of evidence recommends caution in relying on one’s intuitions or even in generalizing from reliable psychological findings to the species, Homo sapiens. Our evolutionary approach suggests that humans have evolved a suite of reliably developing cognitive abilities that adapt our minds, information-processing abilities and emotions ontogenetically to the diverse culturally-constructed worlds we confront.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
While the evidence showing variation in the strength of these illusions across populations remains largely unchallenged, researchers working with nine newly-sighted children have argued that the default, pre-experience, visual calibration in humans generates some detectable susceptibility to the Mueller-Lyer and Ponzo illusions (Gandhi et al. 2015). This may well be the case, but our point is that the perception of, and associated strength of some illusions, varies across populations—not that all illusions are produced only by experience. Nevertheless, this study is beleaguered by relying on a tiny sample, reporting only a crude measure of illusion susceptibility, and failing to explore how long these nearly blind children were sighted earlier in their lives.
Of course, in Europe, we also observe that authority and tradition often implicitly or explicitly served as the justification for particular beliefs and practices. The claim that garlic can diminish the magnetic power of magnets, for example, was passed down over millennia primarily because great ancient Roman philosophers and naturalists wrote about it (Wootton 2016). When it was eventually questioned in the seventeenth century, as modern experimental science was taking shape, we still see a strong epistemic reliance on authority: “…yet I cannot believe that so many famous Writers who have affirmed this perperty of the garlick, could be deceived; therefore I think that they had some other kinde of Load-stone, then that which we have now.” (Ross 1652). Here, in an attempt to preserve the veracity of ancient writers, Ross invokes the auxiliary hypothesis that it must have been a different kind of load stone that the ancients were referring to. By contrast, the political theorist Hannah Arendt (1951, 1961) points out that one defining characteristic of modernity is the loss of authority. Similarly, Strevens argues that since the scientific revolution in the seventeenth century, great emphasis has been placed on experience as the ultimate way to validate knowledge claims and as a result the appeal to other epistemic sources has declined (Wootton 2016).
Miller observed similar effects for prosocial situations, though dispositional attributions are generally less pronounced. See Heine (2012) for discussion.
Interestingly, Miller did find that a Christian community of Indians in Mysore did make more dispositional attributions (30%) than either lower or middle-class members of the Hindu majority. In fact, they were about halfway between the Hindu majority and Chicago. Several researchers have argued that some forms of Christianity encourage dispositional thinking (Cohen and Rozin 2001; Henrich 2020; Li et al. 2012).
Interestingly, there may be genetic variation that influences our ability to perceive specific regions of the color spectrum and this may impact the presence of color terms for ‘blue’ (Brown and Lindsey 2004).
References
Acquaviva, P. (2017). Number in Language. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.61.
Alesina, A.F., P. Giuliano, and N. Nunn. 2013. On the origins of gender roles: Women and the plough. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 128 (2): 469–530.
Almoammer, A., J. Sullivan, C. Donlan, F. Marusic, R. Zaucer, T. O’Donnell, and D. Barner. 2013. Grammatical morphology as a source of early number word meanings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110 (46): 18448–18453. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1313652110.
Annus, A. (2010). Divination and interpretation of signs in the ancient world. In Oriental Institute Seminars.
Apicella, C.L., E.M. Azevedo, N.A. Christakis, and J.H. Fowler. 2014. Evolutionary origins of the endowment effect: Evidence from hunter-gatherers. American Economic Review 104 (6): 1793–1805. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.104.6.1793.
Apicella, C., A. Norenzayan, and J. Henrich. 2020. Beyond WEIRD: A review of the last decade and a look ahead to the global laboratory of the future. Evolution and Human Behavior 41 (5): 319–329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.015.
Arendt, H. 1951. The burden of our time. Secker & Warburg.
Arendt, H. 1961. The concept of history: Ancient and modern. Between Past and Future 89.
Atkinson, Q.D. 2011. Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa. Science 332 (6027): 346–349.
Atran, S., D.L. Medin, and N. Ross. 2004. Evolution and devolution of knowledge: A tale of two biologies. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 10 (2): 395–420.
Awad, E., S. Dsouza, R. Kim, J. Schulz, J. Henrich, A. Shariff, J.-F. Bonnefon, and I. Rahwan. 2018. The moral machine experiment. Nature 563 (7729): 59–64. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0637-6.
Baillargeon, R., R.M. Scott, and L. Bian. 2016. Psychological reasoning in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology 67 (1): 159–186. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115033.
Baimel, A., M. Juda, S. Birch and J. Henrich. 2021. Machiavellian strategist or cultural learner? Mentalizing and learning over development in a resource-sharing game. Evolutionary Human Sciences 3. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2021.11.
Bandura, A. 1977. Social learning theory. Prentice Hall.
Barhorst-Cates, E.M., C. Meneghetti, Y. Zhao, F. Pazzaglia, and S.H. Creem-Regehr. 2021. Effects of home environment structure on navigation preference and performance: A comparison in Veneto, Italy and Utah, USA. Journal of Environmental Psychology 101580.
Barrett, H.C., and J. Broesch. 2012. Prepared social learning about dangerous animals in children. Evolution and Human Behavior 33 (5): 499–508.
Barrett, H.C., T. Broesch, R.M.M. Scott, Z.J. He, R. Baillargeon, D. Wu, M. Bolz, J. Henrich, P. Setoh, J.X. Wang, and S. Laurence. 2013. Early false-belief understanding in traditional non-Western societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 280 (1755): 1–6.
Barrett, H.C., A. Bolyanatz, A.N. Crittenden, D.M.T. Fessler, S. Fitzpatrick, M. Gurven, J. Henrich, M. Kanovsky, G. Kushnick, A. Pisor, B.A. Scelza, S. Stich, C. von Rueden, W.Y. Zhao, and S. Laurence. 2016. Small-scale societies exhibit fundamental variation in the role of intentions in moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113 (17): 4688–4693.
Barwich, A. S. (2020). Smellosophy: What the nose tells the mind. Harvard University Press. https://books.google.com/books?id=7lnhDwAAQBAJ
Becker, A. (n.d.). On the economic origins of restrictions on Women’s sexuality. CESifo working paper, 7770. https://ssrn.com/abstract=3432818
Begg, P.R. 1954. Stone age man’s dentition: With reference to anatomically correct occlusion, the etiology of malocclusion, and a technique for its treatment. American Journal of Orthodontics 40 (7): 517–531.
Bentz, C., T. Soldatova, A. Koplenig and T. Samardžić. 2016. A comparison between morphological complexity measures: typological data vs. language corpora.
Berman, H.J. 1983. Law and revolution: The formation of the Western legal tradition. Harvard University Press.
Berry, J.W. 1966. Temne and Eskimo perceptual skills. International Journal of Psychology 1 (3): 207–229. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207596608247156.
Berry, J.W. 1968. Ecology, perceptual development and the Mueler-Lyer illusion. British Journal of Psychology 59: 205–210.
Birch, L.L. 1987. Children’s food preferences: Developmental patterns and environmental influences. Annals of Child Development 4: 171–208.
Blake, P.R., J. Corbit, T.C. Callaghan, and F. Warneken. 2016. Give as I give: Adult influence on children’s giving in two cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 152: 149–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.010.
Blasi, D.E., S.M. Michaelis, and M. Haspelmath. 2017. Grammars are robustly transmitted even during the emergence of creole languages. Nature Human Behaviour.
Blasi, D.E., S. Moran, S.R. Moisik, P. Widmer, D. Dediu, and B. Bickel. 2019. Human sound systems are shaped by post-Neolithic changes in bite configuration. Science 363 (6432): eaav3218. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aav3218.
Boroditsky, L. 2011. How languages construct time. Space, Time and Number in the Brain: 333–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-385948-8.00020-7.
Boroditsky, L. 2018. Language and the construction of time through space. Trends in Neurosciences 41 (10): 651–653. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2018.08.004.
Bourguignon, E. 1972. Dreams and altered states of consciousness in anthropological research.
Boyd, R. 2017. A different kind of animal: How culture transformed our species. Princeton University Press.
Boyd, R., and P.J. Richerson. 2005. The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford University Press.
Boyd, R., H. Gintis, and S. Bowles. 2010. Coordinated punishment of defectors sustains cooperation and can proliferate when rare. Science 328 (5978): 617–620.
Boyd, R., P.J. Richerson, and J. Henrich. 2011. Rapid cultural adaptation can facilitate the evolution of large-scale cooperation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65 (3): 431–444.
Brady, M., & Fricker, M. (Eds.). (2016). The epistemic life of groups: Essays in the epistemology of collectives. Oxford University Press.
Bramble, D.M., and D.E. Lieberman. 2004. Endurance running and the evolution of Homo. Nature 432 (7015): 345–352.
Bremner, A.J., M.J. Doherty, S. Caparos, J. de Fockert, K.J. Linnell, and J. Davidoff. 2016. Effects of culture and the urban environment on the development of the Ebbinghaus illusion. Child Development 87 (3): 962–981. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12511.
Brown, A.M., and D.T. Lindsey. 2004. Color and language: Worldwide distribution of Daltonism and distinct words for “blue.”. Visual Neuroscience 21: 409–412.
Brownstein, M., and D. Kelly. 2019. Review of the evolution of moral Progress: A biocultural theory by Allen Buchanan and Russell Powell. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science Review of Books 1.
Buchanan, A., and R. Powell 2018. The evolution of moral Progress: A biocultural theory. Oxford University Press. https://books.google.com/books?id=6AJfDwAAQBAJ
Buchtel, E.E., and A. Norenzayan. 2008. Which should you use, intuition or logic? Cultural differences in injunctive norms about reasoning. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 11 (4): 264–273.
Busch, J.T.A., R.E. Watson-Jones, and C.H. Legare. 2018. Cross-cultural variation in the development of folk ecological reasoning. Evolution and Human Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.02.004.
Callaghan, T., P. Rochat, A. Lillard, M.L. Claux, H. Odden, S. Itakura, S. Tapanya, and S. Singh. 2005. Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning—Evidence from five cultures. Psychological Science 16 (5): 378–384.
Campbell, C.E., and B.I. Strassmann. 2016. The blemishes of modern society?: Acne prevalence in the Dogon of Mali. Evolution, Medicine, and Public Health 2016 (1): 325–337. https://doi.org/10.1093/emph/eow027.
Cashdan, E., F.W. Marlowe, A.N. Crittenden, C. Porter, and B.M. Wood. 2012. Sex differences in spatial cognition among Hadza foragers. Evolution and Human Behavior 33 (4): 274–284.
Cashdan, E., K.L. Kramer, H.E. Davis, L. Padilla, and R.D. Greaves. 2016. Mobility and navigation among the Yucatec Maya. Human Nature 27 (1): 35–50.
Chen, M.K. 2021. The effect of language on economic behavior: Evidence from savings rates. Health Behaviors, and Retirement Assets. 43.
Cheng, J.T., C. Anderson, E.R. Tenney, S. Brion, D.A. Moore, and J.M. Logg. 2020. The social transmission of overconfidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000787.
Cheung, B.Y., M. Chudek, and S.J. Heine. 2011. Evidence for a sensitive period for acculturation: Younger immigrants report acculturating at a faster rate. Psychological Science 22 (2): 147–152.
Chiu, C., M.W. Morris, Y. Hong, and T. Menon. 2000. Motivated cultural cognition: The impact of implicit cultural theories on dispositional attribution varies as a function of need for closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78 (2): 247–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.247.
Choi, I., R.E. Nisbett, and A. Norenzayan. 1999. Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin 125 (1): 47–63. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.47.
Chudek, M., and J. Henrich. 2010. Culture-gene coevolution, norm-psychology, and the emergence of human Prosociality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 (5): 218–226.
Chudek, M., P. Brosseau, S. Birch, and J. Henrich. 2013. Culture-gene coevolutionary theory and children’s selective social learning. In The development of social cognition, ed. M. Banaji and S. Gelman. Oxford.
Cohen, A.B., and P. Rozin. 2001. Religion and the morality of mentality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 (4): 697–710. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.4.697.
Cole, M. 1990. Cognitive development and formal schooling: The evidence from cross-cultural research. In In Vygotsky and education: Instructional implications and applications of sociohistorical psychology, 89–110. Cambridge University Press.
Cole, M. 2003. Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline (6. Printing). Belknap Press of Harvard Univ. Press.
Cole, M. (Ed.). (1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking: An exploration in experimental anthropology. Basic Books.
Coluccia, E., and G. Louse. 2004. Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review. Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 (3): 329–340.
Copi, I.M., C. Cohen, and V. Rodych. 2018. Introduction to logic. Routledge.
Cordain, L., S.B. Eaton, J. Brand Miller, S. Lindeberg, and C. Jensen. 2002. An evolutionary analysis of the aetiology and pathogenesis of juvenile-onset myopia. Acta Ophthalmologica Scandinavica 80 (2): 125–135. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0420.2002.800203.x.
Crittenden, A.N., A. Farahani, K.N. Herlosky, T.R. Pollom, I.A. Mabulla, I.T. Ruginski, and E. Cashdan. 2021. Sex differences in harm avoidance and mobility in middle childhood and early adolescence among Hadza foragers. Human Nature.
Curtin, C.M., H.C. Barrett, A. Bolyanatz, A.N. Crittenden, D.M.T. Fessler, S. Fitzpatrick, M. Gurven, M. Kanovsky, G. Kushnick, S. Laurence, A. Pisor, B. Scelza, S. Stich, C. von Rueden, and J. Henrich. 2020. Kinship intensity and the use of mental states in moral judgment across societies. Evolution and Human Behavior 41 (5): 415–429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.002.
Davis, H.E., and E. Cashdan. 2019. Spatial cognition, navigation, and mobility among children in a forager-horticulturalist population, the Tsimané of Bolivia. Cognitive Development 52: 100800. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100800.
Davis, H.E., J. Stack, and E. Cashdan. 2021. Cultural change reduces gender differences in mobility and spatial ability among forager-pastoralist children, the Twa of northern Namibia. Human Nature.
Davis, H.E., M.D. Gurven, and E. Cashdan. in press. Navigational experience and the preservation of spatial abilities into old age among a tropical forager-horticulturalist population. Topics in Cognitive Science.
de Haan, F. 2010. Typology of tense, aspect, and modality systems. In The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199281251.013.0021.
Dembroff, R., and D. Wodak. 2018. He/she/they/ze, 5. Ergo: An Open Access Journal of Philosophy.
Deutscher, G. 2005. The unfolding of language: An evolutionary tour of mankind’s greatest invention (13811382; 1st ed.). Metropolitan Books. http://www.loc.gov/catdir/enhancements/fy0619/2004065607-b.htmlhttp://www.loc.gov/catdir/enhancements/fy0619/2004065607-d.htmlhttp://www.loc.gov/catdir/enhancements/fy0619/2004065607-s.html
Deutscher, G. 2010. Through the language glass: Why the world looks different in other languages (16050008; 1st ed.). Metropolitan Books/Henry Holt and Co.
Edenberg, E., and M. Hannon 2021. Political epistemology. OUP Oxford. https://books.google.com/books?id=ORUuEAAAQBAJ
Enke, B. 2019. Kinship, cooperation, and the evolution of moral systems*. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 134 (2): 953–1019. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjz001.
Ensminger, J., & Henrich, J. (Eds.). (2014). Experimenting with social norms: Fairness and punishment in cross-cultural perspective. Russell Sage Press.
Evans, C.L., K.N. Laland, M. Carpenter, and R.L. Kendal. 2018. Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly “optimal-” rather than “over-” imitators. Developmental Science 21 (5): 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12637.
Everett, D.L. 2005. Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Piraha—Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology 46 (4): 621–646.
Everett, C., D.E. Blasi, and S.G. Roberts. 2015. Climate, vocal folds, and tonal languages: Connecting the physiological and geographic dots. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1417413112.
Everett, C., D.E. Blasi, and S.G. Roberts. 2016. Language evolution and climate: the case of desiccation and tone. Journal of Language Evolution 1 (1): 33–46.
Falk, A., A. Becker, T. Dohmen, B. Enke, D. Huffman, and U. Sunde. 2018. Global Evidence on Economic Preferences*. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 133 (4): 1645–1692. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjy013.
Fausey, C., and L. Boroditsky. 2010. Constructing agency: The role of language. Frontiers in Psychology 1 (162).
Fausey, C., and L. Boroitsky. 2010. Who dunnit? Cross-linguistic differences in eye-witness memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 18 (1): 150–157.
Flad, R.K. 2008. Divination and power. Current Anthropology. https://doi.org/10.1086/588495.
Fodde-Reguer, A.-A. 2014. Divining Bureaucracy: Divination Manuals as Technology and the Standardization of Efficacy in Early China [PhD thesis]. the University of Michigan.
Fodor, J.A. 1983. The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press.
Foley, W.A. 1997. Anthropological linguistics: An introduction. Blackwell Publishers.
Forker, D. 2020. Elevation as a grammatical and semantic category of demonstratives. Frontiers in Psychology 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01712.
Funkhouser, E. 2020. A tribal mind: Beliefs that signal group identity or commitment. Mind & Language. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12326.
Galea, L.A.M., and D. Kimura. 1993. Sex differences in route-learning. Pers. Indiv. Diff. 14 (1): 53–65.
Galor, O., & Sarid, A. (2018). Geographical origins of language structures.
Gandhi, T., A. Kalia, S. Ganesh, and P. Sinha. 2015. Immediate susceptibility to visual illusions after sight onset. Current Biology 25 (9): R358–R359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.005.
Gaulin, S.J., R.W. FitzGerald, and M.S. Wartell. 1990. Sex differences in spatial ability and activity in two vole species (Microtus ochrogaster and M. pennsylvanicus). Journal of Comparative Psychology 104 (1): 88.
Gay, V., D.L. Hicks, E. Santacreu-Vasut, and A. Shoham. 2018. Decomposing culture: An analysis of gender, language, and labor supply in the household. Review of Economics of the Household 16 (4): 879–909. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11150-017-9369-x.
Geary, D.C. 1995. Sexual selection and sex differences in spatial cognition. Learning and Individual Differences 7 (4): 289–301.
Geary, D.C. 2010. Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences, 2nd ed. American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/12072-000.
Gilbert, D.T., and P.S. Malone. 1995. The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin 117: 21–38.
Gislen, A., M. Dacke, R.H.H. Kroger, M. Abrahamsson, D.-E. Nilsson, and E.J. Warrant. 2003. Superior underwater vision in a human population of sea gypsies. Current Biology 13 (10): 833–836.
Giuliano, P. 2018. Gender: A historical perspective. In The Oxford handbook of women and the economy, ed. S.L. Averett, L.M. Argys, and S.D. Hoffman, 644–672. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190628963.013.29.
Giuliano, P., & Nunn, N. (forthcoming). Understanding cultural persistence and change. Review of Economics & Statistics. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01091620.
Gneezy, U., K.L. Leonard, and J.A. List. 2009. Gender differences in competition: Evidence from a matrilineal and a patriarchal society. Econometrica 77 (5): 1637–1664. https://doi.org/10.3982/ECTA6690.
Goldman, A., and C. O’Connor. 2019. Social epistemology. In The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, ed. E. Zalta (p. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2019/entries/epistemology-social/).
Goldstein, J., J. Davidoff, and D. Roberson. 2009. Knowing color terms enhances recognition: Further evidence from English and Himba. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102 (2): 219–238.
Gómez-Robles, A., W.D. Hopkins, S.J. Schapiro, and C.C. Sherwood. 2015. Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112 (48): 14799–14804. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1512646112.
Gong, B., and C.L. Yang. 2012. Gender differences in risk attitudes: Field experiments on the matrilineal Mosuo and the patriarchal Yi. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 83 (1): 59–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.06.010.
Gotti, G., S.G. Roberts, M. Fasan, and C.B. Robertson. 2021. Language in economics and accounting research: The role of linguistic history. The International Journal of Accounting 56 (03): 2150015.
Gurven, M.D., and D.E. Lieberman. 2020. WEIRD bodies: Mismatch, medicine and missing diversity. Evolution and Human Behavior 41 (5): 330–340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.04.001.
Gurven, M., H. Kaplan, J. Winking, D. Eid Rodriguez, S. Vasunilashorn, J.K. Kim, C. Finch, and E. Crimmins. 2009. Inflammation and infection do not promote arterial aging and cardiovascular disease risk factors among lean horticulturalists. PLoS One 4 (8): e6590. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006590.
Gurven, M.D., B.C. Trumble, J. Stieglitz, A.D. Blackwell, D.E. Michalik, C.E. Finch, and H.S. Kaplan. 2016. Cardiovascular disease and type 2 diabetes in evolutionary perspective: A critical role for helminths? Evolution, Medicine and Public Health 2016 (1): 338–357. https://doi.org/10.1093/emph/eow028.
Gurven, M., T.S. Kraft, S. Alami, J.C. Adrian, E.C. Linares, D. Cummings, D.E. Rodriguez, P.L. Hooper, A.V. Jaeggi, R.Q. Gutierrez, I.M. Suarez, E. Seabright, H. Kaplan, J. Stieglitz, and B. Trumble. 2020. Rapidly declining body temperature in a tropical human population. Science Advances 6 (44): eabc6599. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abc6599.
Hamilton, V.L., and J. Sanders. 1992. Everyday justice: Responsibility and the individual in Japan and the United States. Yale University Press.
Hamlin, J.K. 2013a. Failed attempts to help and harm: Intention versus outcome in preverbal infants’ social evaluations. Cognition 128 (3): 451–474.
Hamlin, J.K. 2013b. Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: Evidence for an innate moral Core. Current Directions in Psychological Science 22 (3): 186–193.
Hamlin, J.K., E.V. Hallinan, and A.L. Woodward. 2008. Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others’ goals. Developmental Science 11 (4): 487–494.
Hamlin, J.K., G.E. Newman, and K. Wynn. 2009. Eight-month-old infants infer unfulfilled goals, despite ambiguous physical evidence. Infancy 14 (5): 579–590.
Han, S.H., G. Northoff, K. Vogeley, B.E. Wexler, S. Kitayama, and M.E.W. Varnum. 2013. A cultural neuroscience approach to the biosocial nature of the human brain. Annual Review of Psychology, Vol 64 (64): 335–359.
Hansen, H. (2015). Fallacies.
Harris, P.L., and K.H.H. Corriveau. 2011. Young children’s selective trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366 (1567): 1179–1187.
Hart, R. 1979. Children’s experience of place. Irvington.
Hart, R.A., and G.T. Moore. 1973. The development of spatial cognition: A review. AldineTransaction.
Haun, D.B.M., C.J. Rapold, J. Call, G. Janzen, and S.C. Levinson. 2006. Cognitive cladistics and cultural override in hominid spatial cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103 (46): 17568–17573.
Hay, Jennifer, and L. Bauer. 2007. Phoneme inventory size and population size. Language 83 (2): 388–400. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2007.0071.
Hegarty, M., and D. Waller. 2005. Individual differences in spatial abilities. The Cambridge Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking: 121–169.
Heikinheimo, K., M. Nystrom, T. Heikinheimo, P. Pirttiniemi, and S. Pirinen. 2012. Dental arch width, overbite, and overjet in a Finnish population with normal occlusion between the ages of 7 and 32 years. The European Journal of Orthodontics 34 (4): 418–426.
Heine, S.J. 2012. Cultural psychology. 2nd ed. W.W. Norton.
Heine, S.J. 2016. Cultural psychology. 3rd ed. W. W. Norton & Company, Inc..
Henderson, D. 2020. Are epistemic norms fundamentally social norms? Episteme. https://doi.org/10.1017/epi.2019.49.
Henrich, J. 2004. Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses—The Tasmanian case. American Antiquity 69 (02): 197–214. https://doi.org/10.2307/4128416.
Henrich, J. 2008. A cultural species. In Explaining culture scientifically, ed. M. Brown, 184–210. University of Washington Press.
Henrich, J. 2009. The evolution of innovation-enhancing institutions. In Innovation in cultural systems: Contributions in evolutionary anthropology, ed. S.J. Shennan and M.J. O’Brien, 99–120. MIT.
Henrich, J. 2016. The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.
Henrich, J. 2020. The WEIRDest people in the world: How the west became psychologically peculiar and particularly prosperous. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Henrich, J., and R. Boyd. 2008. Division of labor, economic specialization, and the evolution of social stratification. Current Anthropology 49 (4): 715–724. https://doi.org/10.1086/587889.
Henrich, J., and F.J. Gil-White. 2001. The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 22 (3): 165–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00071-4.
Henrich, N., and J. Henrich. 2007. Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. Oxford University Press.
Henrich, J., and R. McElreath. 2002. Are peasants risk-averse decision makers? Current Anthropology 43 (1): 172–181.
Henrich, J., S.J. Heine, and A. Norenzayan. 2010. The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2–3): 61–83.
Henrich, J., R. Boyd, and P.J. Richerson. 2012. The puzzle of monogamous marriage. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367 (1589): 657–669. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0290.
Henrich, J., M. Chudek, and R. Boyd. 2015. The big man mechanism: How prestige fosters cooperation and creates prosocial leaders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 370 (1683). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0013.
Herculano-Houzel, S. 2019. Life history changes accompany increased numbers of cortical neurons: A new framework for understanding human brain evolution. In Progress in Brain Research (Vol. 250, pp. 179–216). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.06.001.
Heyes, C. 2016. Who knows? Metacognitive social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, xx: 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.12.007.
Hoffman, M., U. Gneezy, and J.A. List. 2011. Nurture affects gender differences in spatial abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108 (36): 14786–14788.
Hollan, D. 1989. The personal use of dream beliefs in the Toraja highlands. Ethos 17 (2): 166–186.
Hong, Z. 2021. Dream interpretation from a cognitive and cultural evolutionary perspective: The case of oneiromancy in traditional China. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/kfzxs.
Hong, Z., and J. Henrich. 2021. The cultural evolution of epistemic Practices: The Case of Divination. Human Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09408-6.
Hong, Z., and Henrich, Joseph. (forthcoming). The cultural evolution of epistemic practices: The case of divination. Human Nature.
Hooven, C. 2021. T: The story of testosterone, the hormone that dominates and divides us. Henry Holt and Company https://books.google.com/books?id=xnb7DwAAQBAJ.
Howe, C.Q., and D. Purves. 2005. The Muller-Lyer illusion explained by the statistics of image-source relationships. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102 (4): 1234–1239. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0409314102.
Hughes, J.D. 2000. Dream interpretation in ancient civilizations. Dreaming. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009447606158.
Jahoda, G. 1966. Geometric illusions and environment: At study in Ghana. British Journal of Psychology 57: 193–199.
Jang, H., C. Boesch, R. Mundry, V. Kandza, and K.R. Janmaat. 2019. Sun, age and test location affect spatial orientation in human foragers in rainforests. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 286 (1907): 20190934.
Ji, L.-J., K. McGeorge, Y. Li, A. Lee, and Z. Zhang. 2015. Culture and gambling fallacies. SpringerPlus 4 (1): 510. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40064-015-1290-2.
Jones, C.M., V.A. Braithwaite, and S.D. Healy. 2003. The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. Behavioral Neuroscience 3 (403–411).
Kahan, D., A.R. Landrum, K. Carpenter, L. Helft, and K.H. Jamieson. 2017. Science curiosity and political information processing. Political Psychology 38: 179–199.
Kahneman, D. 2011. Thinking, fast and slow (16846205; 1st ed.). Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Kaifu, Y., K. Kasai, G.C. Townsend, and L.C. Richards. 2003. Tooth wear and the “design” of the human dentition: A perspective from evolutionary medicine. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 122 (S37): 47–61.
Kauppinen, A. 2018. Epistemic norms and epistemic accountability. Philosopher’s Imprint 18 (8): 1–16.
Kay, P., and T. Regier. 2003. Resolving the question of color naming universals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100 (15): 9085–9089.
Kay, P., and T. Regier. 2006. Language, thought and color: Recent developments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (2): 51–54.
Kitayama, S., K. Yanagisawa, A. Ito, R. Ueda, Y. Uchida, and N. Abe. 2017. Reduced orbitofrontal cortical volume is associated with interdependent self-construal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114 (30): 7969–7974. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704831114.
Kitayama, S., Varnum, M. E., & Salvador, C. E. (2019). Cultural neuroscience.
Kleinfeld, J. 1971. Visual memory in village Eskimo and urban Caucasian children. ARCTIC 24 (2): 132–138. https://doi.org/10.14430/arctic3123.
Kling, A. 2016. Cultural intelligence. National Affairs 27: 150–163.
Knoblock, J. 1988. Xunzi: A translation and study of the complete works (Vol. 1). Stanford University Press.
Koenig, M.A., and M.A. Sabbagh. 2013. Selective social learning: New perspectives on learning from others. Developmental Psychology 49 (3): 399–403.
Kracke, W. 1992. Cultural aspects of dreaming. In Encyclopedia of Dreams. Internationale Institute for Dream Research.
Kuntoro, I.A., L. Saraswati, C. Peterson, and V. Slaughter. 2013. Micro-cultural influences on theory of mind development: A comparative study of middle-class and pemulung children in Jakarta, Indonesia. International Journal of Behavioral Development 37 (3): 266–273. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413478258.
Laland, K.N. 2004. Social learning strategies. Learning & Behavior 32 (1): 4–14.
Laland, K.N. 2017. Darwin’s unfinished symphony: How culture made the human mind. Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400884872.
Levinson, S.C. 2003. Space in language and cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Levy, N., and M. Alfano. 2020. Knowledge from vice: Deeply social epistemology. Mind 129 (515): 887–915. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/fzz017.
Lew-Levy, S., S.M. Kissler, A.H. Boyette, A.N. Crittenden, I.A. Mabulla, and B.S. Hewlett. 2020. Who teaches children to forage? Exploring the primacy of child-to-child teaching among Hadza and BaYaka Hunter-Gatherers of Tanzania and Congo. Evolution and Human Behavior 41 (1): 12–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.07.003.
Li, Y.J., K.A. Johnson, A.B. Cohen, M.J. Williams, E.D. Knowles, and Z. Chen. 2012. Fundamental(ist) attribution error: Protestants are dispositionally focused. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 102 (2): 281–290. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026294.
Lieberman, D.E. 2011. The evolution of the human head. Harvard University Press https://books.google.com/books?id=KvIDVlSExS8C.
Lieberman, D.E. 2012. What we can learn about running from barefoot running: An evolutionary medical perspective. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews 40 (2): 63–72. https://doi.org/10.1097/JES.0b013e31824ab210.
Lieberman, D. 2013. The story of the human body: Evolution, health, and disease. Random House LLC.
Lincoln, J.S. 2003. The dream in native american and other primitive cultures. Courier Corporation.
Lindsey, D.T., A.M. Brown, D.H. Brainard, and C.L. Apicella. 2015. Hunter-gatherer color naming provides new insight into the evolution of color terms. Current Biology 25 (18): 2441–2446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.08.006.
Linn, M.C., and A.C. Petersen. 1985. Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development: 1479–1498.
Littlejohn, C., & Turri, J. (Eds.). (2014). Epistemic norms. Oxford Uniaversity Press.
Locke, J. (1690). An essay concerning human understanding.
Lucy, J.A. 1996. The scope of linguistic relativity: An analysis and review of empirical research.
Luhrmann, T.M., R. Astuti, J. Robbins, J. Cassaniti, J. Marrow, J. Lucy, K. Geurts, J. Throop, A. Vilaça, R. Stasch, B. Rogoff, L. Butler, E. Markman, D. Rutherford, S. Gaskins, G. Jones, M. Karnes, D. Gentner, A. Tran, et al. 2011. Toward an anthropological theory of mind. Suomen Antropologi: Journal of the Finnish Anthropological Society 4: 5–69. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13398-014-0173-7.2.
Luhrmann, T.M., K. Weisman, F. Aulino, J.D. Brahinsky, J.C. Dulin, V.A. Dzokoto, C.H. Legare, M. Lifshitz, E. Ng, N. Ross-Zehnder, and R.E. Smith. 2021. Sensing the presence of gods and spirits across cultures and faiths. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 118 (5): e2016649118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2016649118.
Lupyan, G., and R. Dale. 2010. Language structure is partly determined by social structure. PLoS One 5 (1): e8559.
Luria, A.R. 1976. Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Harvard University Press.
Maddux, W.W., H. Yang, C. Falk, H. Adam, W. Adair, Y. Endo, Z. Carmon, and S.J. Heine. 2010. For whom is parting with possessions more painful? Cultural differences in the endowment effect. Psychological Science 21 (12): 1910–1917. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610388818.
Majid, A., and N. Burenhult. 2014. Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition 130 (2): 266–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.004.
Majid, A., and N. Kruspe. 2018. Hunter-gatherer olfaction is special. Current Biology: 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.014.
Matthews, M.H. 1987. Gender, home range and environmental cognition. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, NS12: 43–56. https://doi.org/10.2307/622576.
McCauley, R., and J. Henrich. 2006. Susceptibility to the Muller-Lyer illusion, theory-neutral observation, and the diachronic penetrability of the visual input system. Philosophical Psychology 19 (1): 1–23.
McElreath, R. 2003. Reputation and the evolution of conflict. Journal of Theoretical Biology 220: 345–357.
McElreath, R., R. Boyd, and P.J. Richerson. 2003. Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology 44 (1): 122–129.
McNamara, R.A., A.K. Willard, A. Norenzayan, and J. Henrich. 2019. Weighing outcome vs. intent across societies: How cultural models of mind shape moral reasoning. Cognition 182: 95–108.
McNamara, R.A., R. Senanayake, A.K. Willard, and J. Henrich. 2021. God’s mind on morality. Evolutionary Human Sciences 3: e6. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2021.1.
McWhorter, J.H. 2008. Our magnificent bastard tongue: The untold history of English. Gotham Books https://books.google.com/books?id=J7rEk4BW0_oC.
Mesoudi, A., L. Chang, K. Murray, and H.J. Lu. 2014. Higher frequency of social learning in China than in the west shows cultural variation in the dynamics of cultural evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 282 (1798).
Miller, J.G. 1984. Culture and development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46 (5): 961–978. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.5.961.
Miller, D.J., T. Duka, C.D. Stimpson, S.J. Schapiro, W.B. Baze, M.J. McArthur, A.J. Fobbs, A.M.M. Sousa, N. Sestan, D.E. Wildman, L. Lipovich, C.W. Kuzawa, P.R. Hof, and C.C. Sherwood. 2012. Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 (41): 16480–16485.
Milton, K. (1988). Foraging behaviour and the evolution of primate intelligence.
Miner, E.J., M. Gurven, H. Kaplan, and S.J.C. Gaulin. 2014. Sex difference in travel is concentrated in adolescence and tracks reproductive interests. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281 (1796): 20141476–20141476. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1476.
Morgan, T.J.H., and K. Laland. 2012. The biological bases of conformity. Frontiers in Neuroscience 6 (87): 1–7.
Muthukrishna, M., and J. Henrich. 2016. Innovation in the collective brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371 (1690): 1–14.
Muthukrishna, M., T.J.H. Morgan, and J. Henrich. 2016. The when and who of social learning and conformist transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 37 (1): 10–20.
Muthukrishna, M., M. Doebeli, M. Chudek, and J. Henrich. 2018a. The cultural brain hypothesis: How culture drives brain expansion, sociality, and life history. PLoS Computational Biology 14 (11): e1006504. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006504.
Muthukrishna, M., J. Henrich, W. Toyokawa, T. Hamamura, T. Kameda, and S.J. Heine. 2018b. Overconfidence is universal? Elicitation of genuine overconfidence (EGO) procedure reveals systematic differences across domain, task knowledge, and incentives in four populations. PLoS One 13 (8): e0202288. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0202288.
Muthukrishna, M., J. Henrich, and E. Slingerland. 2021. Psychology as a historical science. Annual Review of Psychology 72 (1): 717–749. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-082820-111436.
National Science Board. 2018. Science and engineering indicators 2018 (NSB-2018-1). National Science Foundation. https://www.nsf.gov/statistics/indicators/
Nguyen, C.T. 2020. ECHO CHAMBERS AND EPISTEMIC BUBBLES. Episteme 17 (2): 141–161. https://doi.org/10.1017/epi.2018.32.
Nisbett, R.E. 2003. The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Differently...and Why. The Free Press.
Nisbett, R., K. Peng, I. Choi, and A. Norenzayan. 2001. Culture and systems of thought: Holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108 (2): 291–310.
Norenzayan, A., E.E. Smith, B.J. Kim, and R.E. Nisbett. 2002. Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science 26 (5): 653–684.
Park, J., Y. Uchida, and S. Kitayama. 2016. Cultural variation in implicit independence: An extension of Kitayama et al. (). International Journal of Psychology 51 (4): 269–278.
Peng, K., and R.E. Nisbett. 1999. Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist 54 (9): 741–754.
Peters, U. 2020. What is the function of confirmation Bias? Erkenntnis: 1–26.
Pope, S.M., J. Fagot, A. Meguerditchian, D.A. Washburn, and W.D. Hopkins. 2018. Enhanced Cognitive Flexibility in the Seminomadic Himba. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology: 002202211880658. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022118806581.
Powell, R.A., and M.S. Mitchell. 2012. What is a home range? Journal of Mammalogy 93 (4): 948–958.
Protsiv, M., C. Ley, J. Lankester, T. Hastie, and J. Parsonnet. 2020. Decreasing human body temperature in the United States since the industrial revolution. ELife 9: e49555. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.49555.
Proulx, M.J., O.S. Todorov, A. Taylor Aiken, and A.A. de Sousa. 2016. Where am I? Who am I? The relation between spatial cognition, social cognition and individual differences in the built environment. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 64.
Raymond, L., and D. Kelly. forthcoming. Norm-based governance for a new era: Collective action in the face of hyper-politicization. Perspectives on Politics.
Rendell, L., L. Fogarty, W.J.E. Hoppitt, T.J.H. Morgan, M.M. Webster, and K.N. Laland. 2011. Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 (2): 68–76.
Rini, R. 2017. Fake news and partisan epistemology. Kennedy Institute of Ethics Journal 27 (2): E-43.
Rivers, W.H.R. 1901. Introduction and vision. In Reports of the Cambridge anthropological expedition to the Torres Straits: Vol. II, ed. A.C. Haddon. The University Press.
Robbins, J., and A. Rumsey. 2008. Introduction: Cultural and linguistic anthropology and the opacity of other minds. Anthropological Quarterly 81 (2): 407–420.
Robbins, E., J. Shepard, and P. Rochat. 2017. Variations in judgments of intentional action and moral evaluation across eight cultures. Cognition 164: 22–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.02.012.
Roberts, S.G., J. Winters, and K. Chen. 2015. Future tense and economic decisions: Controlling for cultural evolution. PLoS One 10 (7): e0132145. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132145.
Rogoff, B. 1981. Schooling and the development of cognitive skills. Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology 4: 233–294.
Rosenthal, T.L., and B.J. Zimmerman. 1978. Social learning and cognition. Academic Press.
Rudinow, J. 2008. Invitation to critical thinking. Thomson Wadsworth.
Rudy-Hiller, F. 2019. Rudy-Hiller, Fernando, “The Epistemic Condition for Moral Responsibility”,. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2019 Edition). https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/moral-responsibility/
Rushton, J.P. 1975. Generosity in children: Immediate and long term effects of modeling, preaching, and moral judgement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31: 459–466.
Sachdeva, S., P. Singh, and D. Medin. 2011. Culture and the quest for universal principles in moral reasoning. International Journal of Psychology : Journal International de Psychologie 46 (3): 161–176. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2011.568486.
Sacks, O. 2012. The island of the colorblind. Knopf Doubleday Publishing Group https://books.google.com/books?id=9p_RY4ujCTQC.
Salali, G.D., M. Juda, and J. Henrich. 2015. Transmission and development of costly punishment in children. Evolution and Human Behavior 36 (2): 86–94 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.09.004.
Schaffner, B.F., and S. Luks. 2018. Misinformation or expressive responding? What an inauguration crowd can tell us about the source of political misinformation in surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly 82: 135–147.
Schieffelin, B. 1990. The give and take of everyday life: Language socialization of Kaluli children. Cambridge University Press.
Schieffelin, B. 2008. Speaking only your own mind: Reflections on talk, gossip and intentionality in Bosavi (PNG). Anthropological Quarterly 81 (2): 431–441.
Schulz, J.F., D. Bahrami-rad, J.P. Beauchamp, and J. Henrich. 2019. The church, intensive kinship, and global psychological variation. Science 366 (707). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aau5141.
Scribner, S. 1975. Recall of classical syllogisms: A cross-cultural investigation of error on logical problems. In Reasoning: Representation and process, 154–173. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Scribner, S., and M. Cole. 1973. Cognitive consequences of formal and informal education: New accommodations are needed between school-based learning and learning experiences of everyday life. Science 182 (4112): 553–559. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.182.4112.553.
Segall, M., D.T. Campbell, and M.J. Herskovits. 1963. Cultural differences in the perception of geometric illusions. Science 139 (3556): 769–771.
Segall, M., D. Campbell, and M.J. Herskovits. 1966. The influence of culture on visual perception. The Bobbs-Merrill Company.
Selcuk, B., K.A. Brink, M. Ekerim, and H.M. Wellman. 2018. Sequence of theory-of-mind acquisition in Turkish children from diverse social backgrounds. Infant and Child Development 27 (4): 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2098.
Sharp, D., M. Cole, C. Lave, H.P. Ginsburg, A.L. Brown, and L.A. French. 1979. Education and cognitive development: The evidence from experimental research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 44 (1/2): 1. https://doi.org/10.2307/3181586.
Sherwood, C.C., and A. Gómez-Robles. 2017. Brain plasticity and human evolution. Annual Review of Anthropology 46 (1): 399–419. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-102215-100009.
Sherwood, C.C., A.L. Bauernfeind, S. Bianchi, M.A. Raghanti, and P.R. Hof. 2012. Human brain evolution writ large and small. In Progress in brain research (1st ed., Vol. 195). Elsevier B.V. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00011-8.
Silverman, I., J. Choi, and M. Peters. 2007. The hunter-gatherer theory of sex differences in spatial abilities: Data from 40 countries. Archives of Sexual Behavior 36 (2): 261–268.
Singh, M. 2018a. The cultural evolution of shamanism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 41. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17001893.
Singh, M. 2018b. Magic, explanations, and evil: On the origins and design of witches and sorcerers [Preprint]. SocArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/pbwc7.
Smith, K.M., and C.L. Apicella. 2020. Partner choice in human evolution: The role of cooperation, foraging ability, and culture in Hadza campmate preferences. Evolution and Human Behavior 41 (5): 354–366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.009.
Sorokowska, A., P. Sorokowski, T. Hummel, and T. Huanca. 2013. Olfaction and environment: Tsimane’ of Bolivian rainforest have lower threshold of odor detection than industrialized German people. PLoS One 8 (7): e69203. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069203.
Spina, R.R., L.-J. Ji, M. Ross, Y. Li, and Z. Zhang. 2010a. Why best cannot last: Cultural differences in predicting regression toward the mean: Culture and regression. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 13 (3): 153–162. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839X.2010.01310.x.
Spina, R.R., L.-J. Ji, Tieyuan Guo, Zhiyong Zhang, Ye Li, and L. Fabrigar. 2010b. Cultural differences in the representativeness heuristic: Expecting a correspondence in magnitude between cause and effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36 (5): 583–597. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210368278.
Stahl, T., M.P. Zaal, and L.J. Skitka. 2016. Moralized rationality: Relying on logic and evidence in the formation and evaluation of belief can be seen as a moral issue. PLoS One 11 (11): e0166332.
Stich, S. 2006. Is morality an elegant machine or a kludge? Journal of Cognition and Culture 6 (1–2): 181–189. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853706776931349.
Stich, S., and Masaharu Mizumoto. forthcoming. Manifesto. In Epistemology for the rest of the world. Oxford University Press.
Stieglitz, J., M. Gurven, H. Kaplan, and P.L. Hooper. 2013. Household task delegation among high-fertility forager-horticulturalists of lowland Bolivia. Current anthropology 54 (2): 232–241.
Stokes, D. 2018. Attention and the cognitive penetrability of perception. Australasian Journal of Philosophy 96 (2): 303–318.
Street, S.E., A.F. Navarrete, S.M. Reader, and K.N. Laland. 2017. Coevolution of cultural intelligence, extended life history, sociality, and brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114 (30): 7908–7914. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620734114.
Strevens, M. 2020. The knowledge machine: How an unreasonable idea created modern science. Penguin Books Limited https://books.google.com/books?id=zXvNDwAAQBAJ.
Talhelm, T. 2020. Emerging evidence of cultural differences linked to rice versus wheat agriculture. Current Opinion in Psychology 32: 81–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.06.031.
Talhelm, T., X. Zhang, S. Oishi, C. Shimin, D. Duan, X. Lan, and S. Kitayama. 2014. Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science 344: 603–608.
Tarampi, M.R., N. Heydari, and M. Hegarty. 2016. A tale of two types of perspective taking: Sex differences in spatial ability. Psychological Science 27 (11): 1507–1516.
Tedlock, B. 1987. Dreaming: Anthropological and psychological interpretations. CUP Archive.
Thoma, D., and A.E. Tytus. 2018. How cross-linguistic differences in the Grammaticalization of future time reference influence intertemporal choices. Cognitive Science 42 (3): 974–1000. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12525.
Tibana, R.H.W., L.M. Palagi, and J.A.M. Miguel. 2004. Changes in dental arch measurements of Young adults with Normal occlusion—A longitudinal study. Angle Orthodontist 74 (5): 6.
Tomasello, M. 2020. The ontogenetic foundations of epistemic norms. Episteme 17: 301–315.
Trudgill, P. 2011. Sociolinguistic typology: Social determinants of linguistic complexity. Oxford University Press.
Trumble, B.C., S.J.C. Gaulin, M.D. Dunbar, H. Kaplan, and M. Gurven. 2015. No sex or age difference in dead-reckoning ability among Tsimane forager-horticulturalists. Human Nature.
Tuttle, R., D. Webb, E. Weidl, and M. Baksh. 1990. Further Progress on the Laetoli trails. Journal of Archaeological Science 17 (3): 347–362.
Tuttle, R., D. Webb, and M. Baksh. 1991. Laetoli toes and Australopithecus afarensis. Human Evolution 6 (3): 193–200.
Vander Heyden, K.M., M. Huizinga, M. Raijmakers, and J. Jolles. 2017. Children’s representations of another person’s spatial perspective: Different strategies for different viewpoints? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 153: 57–73.
Vashro, L., and E. Cashdan. 2015. Spatial cognition, mobility, and reproductive success in northwestern Namibia. Evolution and Human Behavior 36 (2): 123–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.09.009.
Vashro, L., L. Padilla, and E. Cashdan. 2016. Sex differences in mobility and spatial cognition: A test of the fertility and parental care hypothesis in northwestern Namibia. Human Nature 27 (1): 16–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9247-2.
Vasilyeva, M., and S.F. Lourenco. 2012. Development of spatial cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 3 (3): 349–362.
Vieider, F.M., M. Lefebvre, R. Bouchouicha, T. Chmura, R. Hakimov, M. Krawczyk, and P. Martinsson. 2015. Common components of risk and uncertainty attitudes across contexts and domains: Evidence from 30 countries. Journal of the European Economic Association 13 (3): 421–452. https://doi.org/10.1111/jeea.12102.
Vinden, P.G. 1999. Children’s understanding of mind and emotion: A multi-culture study. In Cognition and emotion (Vol. 13, issue 1, p. 48). https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379357.
Vinden, P.G. 2002. Understanding minds and evidence for belief: A study of Mofu children in Cameroon. International Journal of Behavioral Development 26 (5): 445–452. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250143000391.
Voyer, D., S. Voyer, and M.P. Bryden. 1995. Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin 117: 250.
Walton, D., and M. Koszowy. 2014. Two kinds of Arguments from Authority in the ad Verecundiam Fallacy. Scholarship at UWindsor.
Wang, R.F., and E.S. Spelke. 2002. Human spatial representation: Insights from animals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (9): 376–382.
Wang, Z., R.T. Devine, K.K. Wong, and C. Hughes. 2016. Theory of mind and executive function during middle childhood across cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 149: 6–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.028.
Wellman, H.M., F. Fang, D. Liu, L. Zhu, and G. Liu. 2006. Scaling of theory-of-mind understandings in Chinese children. Psychological Science 17 (12): 1075–1081.
Whiting, B.B., and C.P. Edwards. 1992. Children of different worlds: The formation of social behavior. Harvard University Press.
Wierzbicka, A. 2013. Imprisoned in English: The hazards of English as a default language. Oxford University Press https://books.google.com/books?id=Zr48BAAAQBAJ.
Willard, A.K., and R.A. McNamara. 2019. The minds of God (s) and humans: Differences in mind perception in Fiji and North America. Cognitive Science 43 (1): e12703.
Wootton, D. 2016. Invention of science: A new history of the scientific revolution. https://www.penguin.co.uk/books/132/132992/the-invention-of-science/9780141040837.html
Wundt, W. 1916. Elements of folk psychology: Outlines of a psychological history of the development of mankind. (E. L. Schaub, Trans.). George Allen & Unwin. http://content.apa.org/books/13042-000
Yates, J. F., Lee, J.-W., & Bush, J. G. G. (1997). General knowledge overconfidence: Cross-national variations, response style, and “reality.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70(2), 87–94.
Yates, J.F., J. Lee, H. Shinotsuka, A. Patalano, and W. Sieck. 1998. Cross-cultural variations in probability judgment accuracy: Beyond general knowledge overconfidence? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 74 (2): 89–117.
Yegian, A.K., S.B. Heymsfield, Daniel E. Heymsfield, and D.E. Lieberman. 2021. Historical body temperature records as a population-level ‘thermometer’ of physical activity in the United States. Current Biology 31 (20): R1375–R1376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.09.014.
Yi-long, H. 2020. Court divination and Christianity in the K’Ang-Hsi era. East Asian Science, Technology, and Medicine. https://doi.org/10.1163/26669323-01001001.
Young, L., and R. Saxe. 2008. The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment. NeuroImage 40: 1912–1920. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.057.
Young, L., and R. Saxe. 2009. Innocent intentions: A correlation between forgiveness for accidental harm and neural activity. Neuropsychologia 47: 2065–2072. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.020.
Young, L., F. Cushman, M. Hauser, and R. Saxe. 2007. The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104 (20): 8235–8240.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
None
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Henrich, J., Blasi, D.E., Curtin, C.M. et al. A Cultural Species and its Cognitive Phenotypes: Implications for Philosophy. Rev.Phil.Psych. 14, 349–386 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00612-y
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00612-y