Apperly, I.A., and S.A. Butterfill. 2009. Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review 116 (4): 953–970.
Article
Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. 2000. The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (11): 417–423.
Article
Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. 2011. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology 63 (1): 1–29.
Article
Google Scholar
Beilock, S.L., and T.H. Carr. 2001. On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130 (4): 701–725.
Article
Google Scholar
Binkofski, F., and L.J. Buxbaum. 2013. Two action systems in the human brain. Brain and Language 127 (2): 222–229.
Article
Google Scholar
Block, N. 2014. Seeing-as in the light of vision science. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 89 (3): 560–572.
Article
Google Scholar
Bullier, J., J.D. Schall, and A. Morel. 1996. Functional streams in occipito-frontal connections in the monkey. Behavioural Brain Research 76 (1): 89–97.
Article
Google Scholar
Burge, T. (2010). Origins of objectivity. Oxford University Press, USA.
Butterfill, S. 2007. What are modules and what is their role in development? Mind & Language 22 (4): 450–473.
Article
Google Scholar
Butterfill, S.A., and C. Sinigaglia. 2014. Intention and motor representation in purposive action. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 88 (1): 119–145.
Article
Google Scholar
Camp, E. 2007. Thinking with maps. Philosophical Perspectives 21: 145–182.
Article
Google Scholar
Carey, D.P., M. Harvey, and A.D. Milner. 1996. Visuomotor sensitivity for shape and orientation in a patient with visual form agnosia. Neuropsychologia 34 (5): 329–337.
Article
Google Scholar
Chou, S.-J., Z. Babot, A. Leingärtner, M. Studer, Y. Nakagawa, and D.D.M. O’Leary. 2013. Geniculocortical input drives genetic distinctions between primary and higher-order visual areas. Science 340 (6137): 1239–1242.
Article
Google Scholar
Christensen, W., J. Sutton, and D.J.F. McIlwain. 2016. Cognition in skilled action: Meshed control and the varieties of skill experience. Mind & Language 31 (1): 37–66.
Article
Google Scholar
Clark, D., and R.B. Ivry. 2010. Multiple systems for motor skill learning. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 1 (4): 461–467.
Google Scholar
Cooper, R., and T. Shallice. 2000. Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology 17 (4): 297–338.
Article
Google Scholar
D’Esposito, M., and B.R. Postle. 2015. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Working Memory. Annual Review of Psychology 66 (1): 115–142.
Dehaene, S., and L. Naccache. 2001. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79 (1–2): 1–37.
Article
Google Scholar
Dobzhansky, T. 1962. Mankind Evolving. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Google Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., & Johnson, M. H. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. MIT Press.
Ferretti, G., and S.Z. Caiani. 2019. Solving the Interface Problem Without Translation: The Same Format Thesis. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 100 (1): 301–333.
Article
Google Scholar
Ferstl, E. (2018). Text comprehension. In S.-A. Rueschemeyer & M. G. Gaskell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford University Press.
Fitch, W.T., and M.D. Martins. 2014. Hierarchical processing in music, language, and action: Lashley revisited. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1316 (1): 87–104.
Article
Google Scholar
Fitts, P.M., and M.I. Posner. 1967. Human performance. Belmont: Wadsworth.
Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press.
Fodor, J. A. (2008). LOT 2: The language of thought revisited. OUP Oxford.
Gibson, K.R. 2002. Evolution of human intelligence: The roles of brain size and mental construction. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 59 (1–2): 10–20.
Article
Google Scholar
Gould, S.J., and E.S. Vrba. 1982. Exaptation—A missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8 (1): 4–15.
Article
Google Scholar
Grol, M.J., J. Majdandžić, K.E. Stephan, L. Verhagen, H.C. Dijkerman, H. Bekkering, et al. 2007. Parieto-frontal connectivity during visually guided grasping. Journal of Neuroscience 27 (44): 11877–11887.
Article
Google Scholar
Hallgrímsson, B., & Hall, B. K. (2005). Variation: A central concept in biology. Elsevier.
Haruno, M., D.M. Wolpert, and M. Kawato. 2003. Hierarchical MOSAIC for movement generation. In Excepta Medica international congress series, ed. T. Ono, G. Matsumoto, R.R. Llinas, A. Bethoz, R. Norgren, H. Nishijo, and R. Tamura, vol. 1250. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Google Scholar
Haugeland, J. 1991. Representational genera. In Philosophy and connectionist theory, ed. W. Ramsey, D. Rumelhart, and S. Stich. New York: Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Held, R. 1993. Development of binocular vision revisited. In Brain development and cognition: A reader, ed. M.H. Johnson, 159–166. Oxford: Blackwell.
Google Scholar
Hochstein, S., and M. Ahissar. 2002. View from the top: Hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron 36 (5): 791–804.
Article
Google Scholar
Hubel, D.H., and T.N. Wiesel. 1962. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. The Journal of Physiology 160 (1): 106–154.
Article
Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P.N., R.M. Byrne, and W. Schaeken. 1992. Propositional reasoning by model. Psychological Review 99 (3): 418–439.
Article
Google Scholar
Johnson, Mark H., & Haan, M. de. (2015). Developmental cognitive neuroscience: An introduction. John Wiley & Sons.
Kahneman, D., A. Treisman, and B.J. Gibbs. 1992. The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology 24 (2): 175–219.
Article
Google Scholar
Keele, S.W., R. Ivry, U. Mayr, E. Hazeltine, and H. Heuer. 2003. The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review 110 (2): 316–339.
Article
Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Knudsen, E.I. 2002. Instructed learning in the auditory localization pathway of the barn owl. Nature 417 (6886): 322–328.
Article
Google Scholar
Lashley, K.S. 1951. The problem of serial order in behavior. In Cerebral mechanisms in behavior, ed. L.A. Jeffress, 112–131. New York: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Lee, T.S., and M. Nguyen. 2001. Dynamics of subjective contour formation in the early visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98 (4): 1907–1911.
Article
Google Scholar
Logan, S.W., and M.G. Fischman. 2015. The death of recency: Relationship between end-state comfort and serial position effects in serial recall: Logan and Fischman (2011) revisited. Human Movement Science 44: 11–21.
Article
Google Scholar
Logie, R. H., & Della Sala, S. (2012). Disorders of visuospatial working memory. In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking (pp. 81–120). Cambridge University Press.
MacKay, D.G. 1982. The problems of flexibility, fluency, and speed–accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychological Review 89 (5): 483–506.
Article
Google Scholar
MacKay, D. G. (1987). The Organization of Perception and Action: A theory for language and other cognitive skills. Springer-Verlag.
Masters, R.S.W. 1992. Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology 83 (3): 343–358.
Article
Google Scholar
von Melchner, L., S.L. Pallas, and M. Sur. 2000. Visual behaviour mediated by retinal projections directed to the auditory pathway. Nature 404 (6780): 871–876.
Article
Google Scholar
Melo, D., A. Porto, J.M. Cheverud, and G. Marroig. 2016. Modularity: Genes, development and evolution. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 47: 463–486.
Article
Google Scholar
Mély, D. A., & Serre, T. (2017). Towards a theory of computation in the visual cortex. In Computational and Cognitive Neuroscience of Vision.
Milner, A.D., and M.A. Goodale. 2008. Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46 (3): 774–785.
Article
Google Scholar
Milner, B. 1962. Les troubles de la memoire accompagnant des lesions hippocampiques bilaterales. Physiologie de l’hippocampe 107: 257–272.
Google Scholar
Milner, D., and M. Goodale. 1995. The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Montero, B.G. 2016. Thought in action: Expertise and the conscious mind. Oxford University Press.
Mylopoulos, M., and E. Pacherie. 2017. Intentions and motor representations: The Interface challenge. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 8 (2): 317–336.
Article
Google Scholar
Pacherie, E. 2008. The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework. Cognition 107 (1): 179–217.
Article
Google Scholar
Pisella, L., F. Binkofski, K. Lasek, I. Toni, and Y. Rossetti. 2006. No double-dissociation between optic ataxia and visual agnosia: Multiple sub-streams for multiple visuo-manual integrations. Neuropsychologia 44 (13): 2734–2748.
Article
Google Scholar
Quilty-Dunn, J. 2019. Perceptual Pluralism. Noûs.
Rizzolatti, G., and M. Matelli. 2003. Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: Anatomy and functions. Experimental Brain Research 153 (2): 146–157.
Article
Google Scholar
Rossetti, Y., L. Pisella, and R.D. McIntosh. 2017. Rise and fall of the two visual systems theory. Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine 60 (3): 130–140.
Article
Google Scholar
Roth, G. (2013). The long evolution of brains and minds. Springer Science & Business Media.
Schenk, T., V. Franz, and N. Bruno. 2011. Vision-for-perception and vision-for-action: Which model is compatible with the available psychophysical and neuropsychological data? Vision Research 51 (8): 812–818.
Article
Google Scholar
Schmidt, R.A. 1975. A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review 82 (4): 225–260.
Article
Google Scholar
Sharma, J., A. Angelucci, and M. Sur. 2000. Induction of visual orientation modules in auditory cortex. Nature 404 (6780): 841–847.
Article
Google Scholar
Shepherd, J. 2017. Skilled action and the double life of intention. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 98 (2): 286–305.
Article
Google Scholar
Shepherd, J. 2018. Intelligent action guidance and the use of mixed representational formats. Synthese.
Shin, S.-J. (1994). The logical status of diagrams. Cambridge University Press.
Siegel, S. (2010). The contents of visual experience. Oxford University Press.
Sinigaglia, C., & Butterfill, S. A. (2015). On a puzzle about relations between thought, experience and the motoric. Synthese, 1–14.
Spiegel, M.A., D. Koester, and T. Schack. 2013. The functional role of working memory in the (re-)planning and execution of grasping movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 39 (5): 1326–1339.
Google Scholar
Spunt, R.P., and R. Adolphs. 2017. A new look at domain specificity: Insights from social neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 18 (9): 559–567.
Article
Google Scholar
Squire, L.R. 2009. Memory and brain systems: 1969–2009. Journal of Neuroscience 29 (41): 12711–12716.
Article
Google Scholar
van Polanen, V., & Davare, M. (2015). Interactions between dorsal and ventral streams for controlling skilled grasp. Neuropsychologia, 79, Part B, 186–191.
Verhagen, L., H.C. Dijkerman, W.P. Medendorp, and I. Toni. 2013. Hierarchical Organization of Parietofrontal Circuits during goal-directed action. Journal of Neuroscience 33 (15): 6492–6503.
Article
Google Scholar
Weigelt, M., D.A. Rosenbaum, S. Huelshorst, and T. Schack. 2009. Moving and memorizing: Motor planning modulates the recency effect in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica 132 (1): 68–79.
Article
Google Scholar
Wheeldon, L. R., & Konopka, A. (2018). Spoken word production: Representation, retrieval, and integration. In S.-A. Rueschemeyer & M. G. Gaskell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.
Wolbers, T., R.L. Klatzky, J.M. Loomis, M.G. Wutte, and N.A. Giudice. 2011. Modality-independent coding of spatial layout in the human brain. Current Biology 21 (11): 984–989.
Article
Google Scholar
Wolpert, D.M., Z. Ghahramani, and M.I. Jordan. 1995. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269 (5232): 1880–1882.
Article
Google Scholar
Willingham, D.B. 1998. A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review 105 (3): 558–584.
Article
Google Scholar