Review of Philosophy and Psychology

, Volume 7, Issue 1, pp 181–196 | Cite as

A New Imagery Debate: Enactive and Sensorimotor Accounts

Article

Abstract

Traditionally, the “Imagery Debate” has opposed two main camps: depictivism and descriptivism. This debate has essentially focused on the nature of the internal representations thought to be involved in imagery, without addressing at all the question of action. More recently, a third, “embodied” view is moving the debate into a new phase. The embodied approach focuses on the interdependence of perception, cognition and action, and in its more radical line this approach promotes the idea that perception is not a process involving internal world-models (representations). The anti-representationalist version of the embodied paradigm covers, among others that we shall not discuss here, two quite different positions, namely the enactive approach and sensorimotor theory. Up to now these two anti-representationalist accounts have generally been confounded. In this paper we will argue that despite some important commonalities, enactive and sensorimotor accounts come with distinctive theoretical traits with regard to their approach to imagery. These become manifest when critically examining the role they assign to sensorimotor engagements with the world. We shall argue that enactive and sensorimotor approaches are different in their understanding of the role of embodied action, and these different notions of embodiment lead to different explanatory accounts of perception and imagery. We propose that, due to existing ambiguities in enactivism, the sensorimotor theory is a better framework for a skill-based approach to imagery.

References

  1. Altmann, G.T.M. 2004. Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: The “blank screen paradigm”. Cognition 93(2): B79–B87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barsalou, L.W. 1999. Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(4): 577–660.Google Scholar
  3. Barsalou, L.W. 2008. Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59(1): 617–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barsalou, L.W., W. Kyle Simmons, A.K. Barbey, and C.D. Wilson. 2003. Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(2): 84–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brandt, S.A., and L.W. Stark. 1997. Spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflect the content of the visual scene. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9(1): 27–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chemero, A. 2009. Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  7. Clark, A. 1997. Being there: putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  8. Clark, A. 2008. Supersizing the mind: embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Clark, A., and J. Toribio. 1994. Doing without representing. Synthese 101(3): 401–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Foglia, L., and R. Grush. 2011. The limitations of a purely enactive (Non-representational) account of imagery. Journal of Consciousness Studies 18(5–6): 35–43.Google Scholar
  11. Fourtassi, M., Hajjioui, A., Urquizar, C., Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., and Pisella, L. 2013. Iterative fragmentation of cognitive maps in a visual imagery task. (F. P. de Lange, Ed.) PLoS ONE 8(7).Google Scholar
  12. Gallagher, S. 2005. How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Clarendon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Ganis, G., W.L. Thompson, and S.M. Kosslyn. 2004. Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: An fMRI study. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research 20(2): 226–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Grush, R. 1998. Perception, imagery, and the sensorimotor loop. In A consciousness reader, Eds. Esken, F. and Heckman F.-D. Schoeningh Verlag.Google Scholar
  15. Grush, R. 2004. The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(3): 377–396.Google Scholar
  16. Hebb, D.O. 1968. Concerning imagery. Psychological Review 75(6): 466–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hesslow, G. 2002. Conscious thought as simulation of behaviour and perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(6): 242–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hesslow, G. 2012. The current status of the simulation theory of cognition. Brain Research 1428: 71–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hickerson, R. 2007. Perception as knowing how to act: Alva Noë’s action in perception. Philosophical Psychology 20(4): 505–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Holšánová, J., B. Hedberg, and N. Nilsson. 1999. Visual and verbal focus patterns when describing pictures. In Current Oculomotor research, ed. W. Becker, H. Deubel, and T. Mergner, 303–304. New York: Springer US.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Humphrey, K., and G. Underwood. 2008. Fixation sequences in imagery and in recognition during the processing of pictures of real-world scenes. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2(3): 1–15.Google Scholar
  22. Hutto, D.D., and E. Myin. 2012. Radicalizing enactivism: Basic minds without content. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Intraub, H., and Richardson, M. 1989. Wide-angle memories of close-up scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 15: 179–187.Google Scholar
  24. Johansson, R., and M. Johansson. 2014. Look here, eye movements play a functional role in memory retrieval. Psychological Science 25(1): 236–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Johansson, R., Holšánová, J., and Holmqvist, K. 2005. What do eye movements reveal about mental imagery? Evidence from visual and verbal elicitations. Proceedings of the 27th Cognitive Science conference.Google Scholar
  26. Johansson, R., J. Holšánová, and K. Holmqvist. 2006. Pictures and spoken descriptions elicit similar Eye movements during mental imagery, both in light and in complete darkness. Cognitive Science 30(6): 1053–1079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Johansson, R., Holšánová, J., and Holmqvist, K. 2010. Eye movements during mental imagery are not reenactments of perception. In Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Eds. Ohlsson S. and Catrambone R. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  28. Johansson, R., Holšánová, J., and Holmqvist, K. 2011. The dispersion of eye movements during visual imagery is related to individual differences in spatial imagery ability. In Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 1200–1205. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  29. Johansson, R., J. Holšánová, R. Dewhurst, and K. Holmqvist. 2012. Eye movements during scene recollection have a functional role, but they are not reinstatements of those produced during encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 38(5): 1289–1314.Google Scholar
  30. Kind, A. 2001. Putting the image back in imagination. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 62(1): 85–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kosslyn, S.M. 1980. Image and mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Kosslyn, S. M. 1994. Image and brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  33. Kosslyn, S.M. 2005. Mental images and the brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22(3): 333–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kosslyn, S.M., N.M. Alpert, W.L. Thompson, V. Maljkovic, S.B. Weise, C.F. Chabris, S.E. Hamilton, et al. 1993. Visual mental imagery activates topographically organized visual cortex: PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5(3): 263–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Kosslyn, S.M., W.L. Thompson, I.J. Kim, and N.M. Alpert. 1995. Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex. Nature 378(6556): 496–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kosslyn, S.M., W.L. Thompson, and N.M. Alpert. 1997. Neural systems shared by visual imagery and visual perception: a positron emission tomography study. NeuroImage 6(4): 320–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kosslyn, S.M., A. Pascual-Leone, O. Felician, S. Camposano, J.P. Keenan, W.L. Thompson, G. Ganis, et al. 1999. The role of area 17 in visual imagery: Convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science (New York, N.Y.) 284(5411): 167–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kosslyn, S.M., W.L. Thompson, and G. Ganis. 2006. The case for mental imagery, vol. vi. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Laeng, B., and D. Teodorescu. 2002. Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene. Cognitive Science 26(2): 207–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Laeng, B., I.M. Bloem, S. D’Ascenzo, and L. Tommasi. 2014. Scrutinizing visual images: The role of gaze in mental imagery and memory. Cognition 131(2): 263–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Loughlin, V. 2014. Sensorimotor knowledge and the radical alternative. In Contemporary Sensorimotor Theory, Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics, Eds. Bishop J.M. and Martin A.O., 105–116. Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
  42. Mack, A., and I. Rock. 1998. Inattentional blindness. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  43. Mast, F.W., and S.M. Kosslyn. 2002. Eye movements during visual mental imagery. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(7): 271–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Maturana, H.R., and Varela, F.J. 1992. The tree of knowledge: the biological roots of human understanding. Cognition.Google Scholar
  45. Moulton, S.T., and S.M. Kosslyn. 2009. Imagining predictions: mental imagery as mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences 364(1521): 1273–1280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Neisser, U. 1967. Cognitive psychology. East Norwalk: Appleton.Google Scholar
  47. Noë, A. 2004. Action in perception. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  48. Noë, A. 2006. Experience without the head. In Perceptual experience, ed. T. Gendler and J. Hawthorne, 411–433. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Noë, A. 2010. Vision Without Representation. In N. Gangopadhyay, M. Madary, & F. Spicer (Eds.), Perception, Action, and Consciousness: Sensorimotor Dynamics and Two Visual Systems, 245–256. Oup Oxford.Google Scholar
  50. Noë, A. 2012. Varieties of presence. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Noton, D., and L. Stark. 1971a. Scanpaths in eye movements during pattern perception. Science (New York, N.Y.) 171(3968): 308–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Noton, D., and L. Stark. 1971b. Scanpaths in saccadic eye movements while viewing and recognizing patterns. Vision Research 11(9): 929–942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. O’Regan, J.K. 2011. Why red doesn’t sound like a bell: understanding the feel of consciousness, 1st ed. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. O’Regan, J. K., and N. Block. 2012. Discussion of J. Kevin O’Regan’s “Why Red Doesn’t Sound Like a Bell: Understanding the Feel of Consciousness.” Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3(1): 89–108.Google Scholar
  55. O’Regan, J.K., and A. Noë. 2001. A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(05): 939–973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Pylyshyn, Z.W. 1981. The imagery debate: Analog media vs. Tacit knowledge. Psychological Review 88: 16–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Pylyshyn, Z.W. 2002. Mental imagery: In search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(2): 157–182.Google Scholar
  58. Pylyshyn, Z.W. 2003. Seeing and visualizing: It’s not what you think. Cambridge: A Bradford Book.Google Scholar
  59. Ryle, G. 2009. The concept of mind: 60th anniversary edition. London: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  60. Shapiro, L. 2010. Embodied cognition. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  61. Simons, D.J., and D.T. Levin. 1997. Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1(1): 241–282.Google Scholar
  62. Spivey, M.J., and J.J. Geng. 2001. Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research 65(4): 235–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Spivey, M.J., M.J. Tyler, D.C. Richardson, and E.E. Young. 2000. Eye movements during comprehension of spoken scene descriptions. Proceedings of the 22nd annual conference of the cognitive science society, 487–492. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.Google Scholar
  64. Thomas, N.J.T. 1999. Are theories of imagery theories of imagination? An active perception approach to conscious mental content. Cognitive Science 23(2): 207–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Thomas, N.J.T. 2003. New support for the perceptual activity theory of mental imagery. Resource document : http://www.imagery-imagination.com/newsupa.htm
  66. Thomas, N.J.T. 2009. Visual imagery and consciousness. In Encyclopedia of Consciousness, Ed. W. P. Banks.Google Scholar
  67. Thomas, N.J.T. 2010. Imagery and the coherence of imagination. Journal of Philosophical Research 22: 95–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Thomas, N. J. T. 2014). Mental imagery. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Ed. E.N. Zalta. Resource document : http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2014/entries/mental-imagery/
  69. Thompson, E. 2007. Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  70. Thompson, E., and F.J. Varela. 2001. Radical embodiment: Neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5(10): 418–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Varela, F., E. Thompson, and E. Rosch. 1991. The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  72. White, A.R. 1990. The language of imagination. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Laboratoire Psychologie de la PerceptionCNRS - Université Paris DescartesParisFrance

Personalised recommendations