Skip to main content
Log in

The Cognitive Design of Tools of Thought

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When thought overwhelms the mind, the mind puts it into the world, notably in diagrams and gestures.Both use space and arrays of elements, depictive and non-depictive, to convey ideas, concrete and abstract,clear and sketchy. The arrays and the non-depictive elements like boxes and arrows serve to showrelationships and organizations, thematic, categorical, and more. on paper, in the air, in the diagrammedworld. Human actions organize space to convey abstractions: spraction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Azema, M., and F. Rivere. 2012. Animation in Paleolithic art: A pre-echo of cinema. Antiquity 86: 316–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bender, J., and M. Marrinan. 2010. The culture of diagram. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bilda, Z and Gero, J.S. (2006) Reasoning with internal and external representations: A case study with expert architects. In R Sun and N. Miyake (Editors), Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, Lawrence Erlbaum, Pp. 1020–1026.

  • Boone, E. H. (2010). Stories in Red and Black: Pictorial Histories of the Aztec and Mixtec. University of Texas Press

  • Brew, K. and Guerra, R. (2013). Design is One: Lella and Massimo Vignelli. Film. US distributor www.firstrunfeatures.com

  • Brown, L. 1979. The story of maps. NY: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casati, R., and A. Varzi. 1996. Introduction. In Events, ed. R. Casati and A. Varzi. Aldershot: Dartmouth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chu, M., and S. Kita. 2008. Spontaneous gestures during mental rotation tasks: Insights into the microdevelopment of the motor strategy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137: 706–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denis, M. 1997. The description of routes: A cognitive approach to the production of spatial discourse. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 16: 409–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denis, M. Pazzaglia, F., Cornoldi, C., & Bertolo, L. (1998). Spatial discourse and navigation: An analysis of route directions in the city of Venice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12

  • Donald, M. 1991. Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egoff, D.B., and L.E. Corder. 1991. Height differences of low and high job status, female and male corporate employees. Sex Roles 24: 365–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K., B. Tversky, and H. Taylor. 2000. Using space to describe space: Perspective in speech, sign, and gesture. Spatial Cognition and Computation 2: 157–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enfield, N.J. 2003. Producing and editing diagrams using co-speech gesture: Spatializing non-spatial relations in explanations of kinship in Laos. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 13(1): 7–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, N.A. 1976. Languages of art: An approach to a theory of symbols. Indianopolis: Hackett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heiser, J., and B. Tversky. 2006. Arrows in comprehending and producing mechanical diagrams. Cognitive Science 30: 581–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heiser, J., B. Tversky, and M. Silverman. 2004. Sketches for and from collaboration. In Visual and spatial reasoning in design III, ed. J.S. Gero, B. Tversky, and T. Knight, 69–78. Sydney: Key Centre for Design Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. 1995. Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jamalian, A., and B. Tversky. 2012. Gestures alter thinking about time. In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. N. Miyake, D. Peebles, and R.P. Cooper, 551–557. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jamalian, A., V. Giardino, and B. Tversky. 2013. Gestures for thinking. In Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sabaenz, and I. Wachsmuth. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. 2006. The ghost map. London: Riverhead Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jurafsky, D. (2013). Why ice cream sounds fat and crackers sound skinny. Stanford Magazine, July/August

  • Kang, S., B. Tversky, and J.B. Black. 2012. From hands to minds: How gestures promote action understanding. In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. N. Miyake, D. Peebles, and R.P. Cooper, 551–557. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kang, S., Tversky, B. & Black, J. B. (2014) Gesture and speech in explanations to experts and novices.

  • Kantrowitz, A. (2014). A cognitive ethnographic study of improvisational drawing by eight contemporary artists. Doctoral dissertation, Columbia Teachers College

  • Kirsh, D. 1995. The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence 73: 31–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. 2013. How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloud, S. 1993. Understanding comics. Northhampton: Kitchen Sink.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A., and P.N. Johnson-Laird. 1976. Language and perception. NY: Belnap Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Netz, R. 1999. Linguistic formulae as cognitive tools. Pragmatics and Cognition 7: 147–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A. 1993. Things that make us smart. Boston: Addison-Wesley Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pike, A.W.G., D.L. Hoffmann, M. Garcia-Diez, P.B. Pettitt, J. Alcolea, R. De Balb, C. Gonzalez-Sainz, C. de las Heras, J.A. Lasheras, R. Motes, and J. Zilhao. 2012. U-Series dating of Paleolithic art in 11 caves in Spain. Science 336: 1409–1413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sadier, B., J.-J. Delannoy, L. Benedetti, D. Bourles, S. Jaillet, J.-M. Geneste, A.-E. Lebatard, and M. Arnold. 2012. Further constraints on the Chauvet cave artwork elaboration. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 8002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schon, D.A. 1983. The reflective practitioner. NY: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, T., and A. Maass (eds.). 2011. Spatial dimensions of social thought. Amsterdam: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R.N. 1984. Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review 91: 417–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, J.P. 1999. Time in space: Narrative in classical art. Art Bulletin 81: 562–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S.M. 1995. Getting into and out of mental ruts: A theory of fixation, incubation, and insight. In The nature of insight, ed. R.J. Sternberg and J.E. Davidson, 229–251. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stjernfelt, F. 2011. Diagrammatology: An investigation on the borderlines of phenomenology, ontology, and semiotics. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suchman, L. 1987. Plans and situated actions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suwa, M., and B. Tversky. 1996. What architects see in their sketches: Implications for design tools. Human factors in computing systems: Conference companion (pp. 191–192). NY: ACM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suwa, M., and B. Tversky. 2001. Constructive perception in design. In Computational and cognitive models of creative design V, ed. J.S. Gero and M.L. Maher, 227–239. Sydney: University of Sydney.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suwa, M., and B. Tversky. 2003. Constructive perception: A skill for coordinating perception and conception. In Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society Meetings, ed. R. Alterman and D. Kirsh. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suwa, M., B. Tversky, J. Gero, and T. Purcell. 2001. Seeing into sketches: Regrouping parts encourages new interpretations. In Visual and spatial reasoning in design, ed. J.S. Gero, B. Tversky, and T. Purcell, 207–219. Sydney: Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. 1983. How language structures space. In Spatial orientation: Theory, research and application, ed. H.L. Pick Jr. and L.P. Acredolo, 225–282. N. Y.: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics. Vols 1 & 2. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. 1981. Distortions in memory for maps. Cognitive Psychology 13: 407–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. 1990. Where partonomies and taxonomies meet. In Meanings and prototypes: Studies on linguistic categorization, ed. S.L. Tsohatzidis, 334–344. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. 1995. Cognitive origins of graphic conventions. In Understanding images, ed. F.T. Marchese, 29–53. New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. 2001. Spatial schemas in depictions. In Spatial schemas and abstract thought, ed. M. Gattis, 79–111. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. 2004. Semantics, syntax, and pragmatics of graphics. In Language and visualisation, ed. K. Holmqvist and Y. Ericsson, 141–158. Lund: Lund University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. 2005. How to get around by mind and body: Spatial thought, spatial action. In Cognition, evolution, and rationality: A cognitive science for the XXIst century, ed. A. Zilhao. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. 2011a. Spatial thought, social thought. In Spatial dimensions of social thought, ed. T. Schubert and A. Maass. Amsterdam: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. 2011b. Visualizations of thought. Topics in Cognitive Science 3: 499–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., and K. Hemenway. 1983. Categories of scenes. Cognitive Psychology 15: 121–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. and Kessell, A. (2014). Thinking in action. Topics in Cognitive Science.

  • Tversky, B., and P.U. Lee. 1998. How space structures language. In Spatial Cognition: An interdisciplinary approach to representation and processing of spatial knowledge, ed. C. Freksa, C. Habel, and K.F. Wender, 157–175. Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., and P.U. Lee. 1999. Pictorial and verbal tools for conveying routes. In Spatial information theory: cognitive and computational foundations of geographic information science, ed. C. Freksa and D.M. Mark, 51–64. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., and M. Suwa. 2009. Thinking with sketches. In Tools for innovation, ed. A. Markman. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., and J.M. Zacks. 2013. Event perception. In Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology, ed. D. Riesberg. Oxford: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., S. Kugelmass, and A. Winter. 1991. Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology 23: 515–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., J. Zacks, P.U. Lee, and J. Heiser. 2000. Lines, blobs, crosses, and arrows: Diagrammatic communication with schematic figures. In Theory and application of diagrams, ed. M. Anderson, P. Cheng, and V. Haarslev, 221–230. Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., J. Heiser, P. Lee, and M.-P. Daniel. 2009. Explanations in gesture, diagram, and word. In Spatial language and dialogue, ed. K.R. Coventry, T. Tenbrink, and J.A. Bateman, 119–131. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., J. Heiser, and J. Morrison. 2013. Space, time, and story. In The psychology of learning and motivation, ed. B.H. Ross, 47–76. Oxford: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wexler, M., S.M. Kosslyn, and A. Berthoz. 1998. Motor processes in mental rotation. Cognition 68: 77–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlschläger, A., and A. Wohlschläger. 1998. Mental and manual rotatation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24: 397–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zacks, J., and B. Tversky. 1999. Bars and lines: A study of graphic communication. Memory and Cognition 27: 1073–1079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zacks, J., and B. Tversky. 2001. Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin 127: 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am indebted to my colleagues and collaborators, especially Valeria Giardino, Jocelyn Penny Small, Azadeh Jamalian, Angela Kessell, Julie Heiser, Paul Lee, and Jeff Zacks. I am grateful the Varieties of Understanding Project at Fordham University and The John Templeton Foundation and to the following National Science Foundation grants for facilitating the research and/or preparing the manuscript: National Science Foundation HHC 0905417, IIS-0725223, IIS-0855995, and REC 0440103. The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views of the funders.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Barbara Tversky.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tversky, B. The Cognitive Design of Tools of Thought. Rev.Phil.Psych. 6, 99–116 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-014-0214-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-014-0214-3

Keywords

Navigation