Review of Philosophy and Psychology

, Volume 4, Issue 2, pp 271–292 | Cite as

Background Emotions, Proximity and Distributed Emotion Regulation

Article

Abstract

In this paper, we draw on developmental findings to provide a nuanced understanding of background emotions, particularly those in depression. We demonstrate how they reflect our basic proximity (feeling of interpersonal connectedness) to others and defend both a phenomenological and a functional claim. First, we substantiate a conjecture by Fonagy & Target (International Journal of Psychoanalysis 88(4):917–937, 2007) that an important phenomenological aspect of depression is the experiential recreation of the infantile loss of proximity to significant others. Second, we argue that proximity has a particular cognitive function that allows individuals to morph into a cohesive dyadic system able to carry out distributed emotion regulation. We show that elevated levels of psychological suffering connected to depressive background emotions may be explained not only in terms of a psychological loss, but also as the felt inability to enter into dyadic regulatory relations with others—an experiential constraint that decreases the individual’s ability to adapt to demanding situations.

References

  1. Alberts, E., A.F. Kalverboer, and B. Hopkins. 1983. Mother-infant dialogue in the first days of life: an observational study during breast-feeding. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 24(1): 145–161.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, V. 1998. Assessing executive functions in children: biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 8: 319–349.Google Scholar
  3. Beebe, B., and F.M. Lachmann. 1998. Co-constructing inner and relational processes: self-and mutual regulation in infant research and adult treatment. Psychoanalytic Psychology 15(4): 480–516.Google Scholar
  4. Beebe, B., J. Jaffe, S. Feldstein, K. Mays, and D. Alson. 1985. Inter-personal timing: The application of an adult dialogue model to mother-infant vocal and kinetic interactions. In Social perception in infants, ed. T.M. Field and N. Fox. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  5. Bernieri, F.J., and R. Rosenthal. 1991. Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior: Studies in emotion & social interaction, ed. R.S. Feldman and B. Rime, 401–432. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Binswanger, Ludwig. 1960. Melancholie und Manie. Phenomenologische Studien. Stuttgart: Neske.Google Scholar
  7. Brazelton, T., B. Koslowski, and M. Main. 1974. The origins or reciprocity. In The effects of the infant on its caregiver, ed. M. Lewis and L. Rosenblum, 137–154. New York: Wiley-Interscience.Google Scholar
  8. Carpendale, J.I.M., and C. Lewis. 2006. How children develop social understanding. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
  9. Chartrand, T.L., and J.A. Bargh. 1999. The chameleon effect: the perception– behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76: 893–910.Google Scholar
  10. Cohn, J.E., and E.Z. Tronick. 1988. Mother-infant face-to-face interaction: influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles in either partner’s behavior. Developmental Psychology 24: 386–392.Google Scholar
  11. Condon, W.S., and L.W. Sander. 1974. Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech. Integrated participation and language acquisition. Science 183: 99.Google Scholar
  12. Dales, S., and P. Jerry. 2008. Attachment, affect regulation and mutual synchrony in adult psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychotherapy 62(3): 283–312.Google Scholar
  13. Damasio, Antonio. 1994. Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons.Google Scholar
  14. Damasio, Antonio. 1999. The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace and Co.Google Scholar
  15. Damasio, A. 2004. Looking for spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
  16. Decety, J. 2002. Naturaliser l’empathie. L’Encéphale 28: 9–20.Google Scholar
  17. Diamond, L.M. 2001. Contributions of psychophysiology to research on adult attachment: review and recommendations. Personality and Social Psychology Review 5: 276–295.Google Scholar
  18. Diamond, L.M., and L.G. Aspinwall. 2003. Integrating diverse developmental perspectives on emotion regulation. Motivation and Emotion 27: 1–6.Google Scholar
  19. Draghi-Lorenz, R., V. Reddy, and A. Costall. 2001. Rethinking the development of ‘non-basic’ emotions: a critical review of existing theories. Developmental Review 21: 263–304.Google Scholar
  20. Drummond, John J. 2004. ‘Cognitive Impenetrability’ and the complex intentionality of the emotions. Journal of Consciousness Studies 11(10–11): 109–126.Google Scholar
  21. Feldman, R. 2007. On the origins of background emotions: from affect synchrony to symbolic expression. Emotion 7(3): 601–611.Google Scholar
  22. Feldman, R., and A.I. Eidelman. 2004. Parent-infant synchrony and the socialemotional development of triplets. Developmental Psychology 40: 1133–1147.Google Scholar
  23. Fonagy, P., and M. Target. 2007. A theory of external reality rooted in intersubjectivity. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis 88(4): 917–937.Google Scholar
  24. Fosha, D. 2001. The dyadic regulation of affect. Journal of Clinical Psychology 57: 227–242.Google Scholar
  25. Fuchs, T. 2005. Corporealized and disembodied minds: a phenomenological view of the body in melancholia and schizophrenia. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology 12: 95–107.Google Scholar
  26. Gallagher, S. 2005. How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Gallagher, S., and A.N. Meltzoff. 1996. The earliest sense of self and others: Merleau-Ponty and recent developmental studies. Philosophical Psychology 9(2): 211–233.Google Scholar
  28. Geerts, E., and A.L. Bouhuys. 1998. Multi-level prediction of short-term outcome of depression: the role of nonverbal interpersonal and cognitive processes and of personality traits. Psychiatry Research 79: 59–72.Google Scholar
  29. Geerts, Erwin, Netty Bouhuys, and Rutger H. Van den Hoofdakker. 1996. Nonverbal attunement between depressed patients and an interviewer predicts subsequent improvement. Journal of Affective Disorders 40(1–2): 15–21.Google Scholar
  30. Geerts, E., T. van Os, J. Ormel, and N. Bouhuys. 2006. Nonverbal behavioral similarity between patients with depression in remission and interviewers in relation to satisfaction and recurrence of depression. Depression and Anxiety 23(4): 200–209.Google Scholar
  31. Gopnik, A. 2009. The philosophical baby: What children’s minds tell Us about truth, love, and the meaning of life. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.Google Scholar
  32. Hazan, C., and P. Shaver. 1987. Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52: 511–524.Google Scholar
  33. Heidegger, M. 1962. Sein und Zeit. Tübingen: Niedermeyer.Google Scholar
  34. Heimann, M. 2002. Notes on individual differences and the assumed elusiveness of neonatal imitation. In The imitativemind: Development, evolution, and brain bases, ed. A.N. Meltzoff and W. Prinz, 74–84. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Henrich, D. 2007. Denken und selbstsein: Vorlesungen über subjektivität. Suhrkamp: Frankfurt am Main.Google Scholar
  36. Hobson, R.P. 1989. Beyond cognition: A theory of autism. In Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment, ed. G. Dawson, 22–48. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  37. Hobson, R.P. 2002. The cradle of thought: Exploring the origins of thinking. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  38. Hobson, P. (2006). Radical enactivism: From feeling to thinking. In Intentionality, phenomenology, and narrative: Focus on the philosophy of Daniel D. Hutto, ed. R. Menary, 179–184. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  39. Hobson, J., and P. Hoson. 2007. Identification: the missing link between joint attention and imitation? Development and Psychopathology 19: 411–431.Google Scholar
  40. Hobson, R.P., and J.A. Meyer. 2005. Foundations for self and other: a study in autism. Developmental Science 8: 481–491.Google Scholar
  41. Hobson, Peter. 2005. What puts the jointness into joint attention?. In Joint attention: Communication and other minds, ed. Naomi Eilan, Christoph Hoerl, Teresa McCormack, and Johannes Roessler, 185 – 204.Google Scholar
  42. Hobson, R.P., M.P.H. Patrick, L.E. Crandell, R.M. García Pérez, and A. Lee. 2004. Maternal sensitivity and infant triadic communication. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45: 470–480.Google Scholar
  43. Hofer, M.A. 1994. Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. In The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations. ed. N.A. Fox. Monographs of the society for research in child development 59(2–3), pp. 250–283.Google Scholar
  44. Jones, S.S. 2009. The development of imitation in infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364(1528): 2325–2335.Google Scholar
  45. Julien, Danielle, Mathilde Brault, Élise Chartrand, and Jean Bégin. 2000. Immediacy behaviours and synchrony in satisfied and dissatisfied couples. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement 32(2): 84–90.Google Scholar
  46. Karp, D. 1996. Speaking of sadness: Depression, disconnection, and the meanings of illness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Kaye, K., and A.J. Wells. 1980. Mothers’ jiggling and the burst–pause pattern in neonatal feeding. Infant Behavior & Development 3: 29–46.Google Scholar
  48. Kraus, A. 1977. Sozialverhalten und psychose manisch-depressiver. Stuttgart: Enke.Google Scholar
  49. Krueger, J. 2011. Extended cognition and the space of social interaction. Consciousness and Cognition 20(3): 643–657.Google Scholar
  50. Kugiumutzakis, G. 1999. Genesis and development of early infant mimesis to facial and vocal models. In Imitation in infancy, ed. J. Nadel and G. Butterworth, 127–185. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Kugiumutzakis, G., T. Kokkinaki, M. Makrodimitraki, and E. Vitalaki. 2005. Emotions in early mimesis. In Emotional development: Recent research advances, ed. J. Nadel and D. Muir, 161–182. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  52. Lakoff, G., and M. Johnson. 1999. Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  53. Lambie, J.A., and A.J. Marcel. 2002. Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: a theoretical framework. Psychological Review 109: 219–259.Google Scholar
  54. Lane, R.D., and L. Nadel. 2000. Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
  55. Laurent, H.K., and S.I. Powers. 2007. Emotion regulation in emerging adult couples: temperament, attachment, and HPA response to conflict. Biological Psychology 76: 61–71.Google Scholar
  56. Mahler, M., S. Pine, and A. Bergman. 1975. The psychological birth of the human infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  57. Manian, N., and M.H. Bornstein. 2009. Dynamics of emotion regulation in infants of clinically depressed and nondepressed mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50(11): 1410–1418.Google Scholar
  58. Martin, J., and L. Failows. 2010. Executive Function: Theoretical Concerns. In Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions, ed. B. Sokol, U. Muller, J.I.M. Carpendale, A.R. Young, and G. Iarocci, 35–55. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Meltzoff, A.N. 2006. The ‘like me’ framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional agent. Acta Psychologica 124: 26–43.Google Scholar
  60. Meltzoff, A.N., and R. Brooks. 2007. Intersubjectivity before language: Three windows on preverbal sharing. In On being moved: From mirror neurons to empathy, ed. S. Braten, 149–174. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  61. Meltzoff, A., and M.K. Moore. 1977. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198: 75–78.Google Scholar
  62. Meltzoff, A., and M.K. Moore. 1989. Imitation in newborn infants: exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology 25: 954–962.Google Scholar
  63. Meltzoff, A., and M. K. Moore. 1997. Explaining facial imitation: a theoretical model, Early Dev. Parent. 6: 179–192.Google Scholar
  64. Merleau-Ponty, M. 1962. Phenomenology of Perception (Trans. Smith, C.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  65. Miles, L.K., J.L. Griffiths, M.J. Richardson, and N.C. Macrae. 2010. Too late to coordinate: target antipathy impedes behavioral synchrony. European Journal of Social Psychology 40: 52–60.Google Scholar
  66. Miller, M.C. 2011. Understanding grief and loss. Bereavement shares much in common with depression. Harv Ment Health Lett. 28(6).Google Scholar
  67. Minkowski, E. 1970. Lived time: Phenomenological and psychopathological studies. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  68. Muratori, F., and S. Maestro. 2007. Autism as a downstream effect of primary difficulties in intersubjectivity interacting with abnormal development of brain connectivity. International Journal for Dialogical Science 2(1): 93–118.Google Scholar
  69. Murray, L., and C. Trevarthen, 1985. Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Social Perception in Infants ed. T. M. Field & N. A. Fox, 177–197. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishers.Google Scholar
  70. Noë, A. 2004. Action in perception. Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  71. Piaget, J. 1954. The Construction of Reality in the Child. (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  72. Pietromonaco, P.R., L.F. Barrett, and S.A. Powers. 2006. Adult attachment theory and affective reactivity and regulation. In Emotion regulation in families and close relationships: Pathways to dysfunction and health, ed. J. Simpson, D. Snyder, and J. Hughes, 57–74. Washington: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  73. Posner, M., and M.K. Rothbart. 1998. Attention, self-regulation, and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 353: 1915–1927.Google Scholar
  74. Premack, D., and G. Woodruff. 1978. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1: 515–526.Google Scholar
  75. Ramseyer, F., and W. Tschacher. 2011. Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: relationship quality and outcome are reflected by coordinated body-movement. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 79: 284–295.Google Scholar
  76. Ratcliffe, M. 2005. The feeling of being. Journal of Consciousness Studies 12(8–10): 43–60.Google Scholar
  77. Ratcliffe, M. 2008. Feelings of being: Phenomenology, psychiatry and the sense of reality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  78. Ratcliffe, M. 2009. Understanding existential changes in psychiatric illness: The indispensability of phenomenology. In Psychiatry as cognitive neuroscience, ed. M. Broome and L. Bortolotti, 223–244. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  79. Ratcliffe, M. 2010. The phenomenology of mood and the meaning of life’. In Oxford handbook on emotion, ed. P. Goldie, 349–371. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  80. Ray, E., and C. Heyes. 2011. Imitation in infancy: the wealth of the stimulus. Developmental Science 14(1): 92–105.Google Scholar
  81. Reck, C., A. Hunt, T. Fuchs, R. Weiss, A. Noon, E. Moehler, G. Downing, E.Z. Tronick, and C. Mundt. 2004. Interactive regulation of affect in postpartum depressed mothers and their infants: an overview. Psychopathology 37: 272–280.Google Scholar
  82. Reddy, V. 2008. How infants know minds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  83. Rochat, P. 2004. The emergence of self-awareness as co-awareness in early child development. In Development of self-consciousness: Interdisciplinary perspectives, ed. D. Zahavi, T. Grunbaum, and J. Parnas, 1–20. The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  84. Rochat, P. 2009. Others in mind – social origins of self-consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  85. Rochat, P., and T. Striano. 1999. Emerging self–exploration by 2–month–old infants. Developmental Science 2(2): 206–218.Google Scholar
  86. Rochat, P., J.G. Querido, and T. Striano. 1999. Emerging sensitivity to the timing and structure of protoconversation in early infancy. Developmental Psychology 35(4): 950–957. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.950.Google Scholar
  87. Rohrbaugh, M.J., M.R. Mehl, V. Shoham, E.S. Reilly, and G.A. Ewy. 2008. Prognostic significance of spouse we talk in couples coping with heart failure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 76: 781–789.Google Scholar
  88. Rothbart, M.K. 1989. Temperament and development. In Temperament in childhood, ed. G.A. Kohnstamm, J.E. Bates, and M.K. Rothbart. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  89. Sartre, J.P. 2008. Sketch for a theory of the emotions. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  90. Saxbe, D., and R.L. Repetti. 2010. For better or worse? Coregulation of couples’ cortisol levels and mood states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 98(1): 92–103.Google Scholar
  91. Sbarra, D.A., and C. Hazan. 2008. Coregulation, dysregulation, self-regulation: an integrative analysis and empirical agenda for understanding adult attachment, separation, loss and recovery. Personality and Social Psychology Review 12: 141–167.Google Scholar
  92. Schore, A.N. 2003. Affect regulation and the repair of the self. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  93. Schore, J.N., and A.N. Schore. 2008. Modern attachment theory: the central role of affect regulation in development and treatment. Journal of Clinical Social Work 36: 9–20.Google Scholar
  94. Shaw, R. 2004. The embodied psychotherapist: an exploration of the therapists’ somatic phenomena within the therapeutic encounter. Psychotherapy Research 14: 271–288.Google Scholar
  95. Shoebi, D. 2008. The coregulation of daily affect in marital relationships. Journal of Family Psychology 22(4): 595–604.Google Scholar
  96. Slaby, J., and A. Stephan. 2008. Affective intentionality and self-consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 17(2): 506–513.Google Scholar
  97. Solomon, A. 2001. The noonday demon: An atlas of depression. New York: Scribner.Google Scholar
  98. Stanghellini, G. 2000. Phenomenology of the social self of the schizotype and the melancholic type. In Exploring the self, ed. D. Zahavi, 279–294. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  99. Stanghellini, G. 2004. Disembodied spirits and deanimated bodies: The psychopathology of common sense. OUP Oxford.Google Scholar
  100. Steinbock, A. 2007. A phenomenology of despair. International Journal of Philosophical Studies 15(3): 435–451.Google Scholar
  101. Stern, D. 1985. The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  102. Stern, D. 2010. Forms of vitality: Exploring dynamic experience in psychology, the arts, psychotherapy, and development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  103. Striano, T., and P. Rochat. 1999. Developmental link between dyadic and triadic social competence in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 17(4): 551–562.Google Scholar
  104. Tellenbach, Hubertus. 1961. Melancholie. Zur problemgeschichte, typologie, pathogenese und klinik. Mit einem geleitwort von V. E. von gebsattel. Berlin, Göttingen, Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
  105. Thornhill, R. and N.W. Thornhill. 1989. “The evolution of psychological pain”. Sociobiology and the Social Sciences 73–103.Google Scholar
  106. Tomasello, M. 1999. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  107. Trehub, S.E., and L.J. Trainor. 1993. Listening strategies in infancy: The roots of music and language development. In Thinking in sound: The cognitive psychology of human audition, ed. S. McAdams and E. Bigand. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  108. Trehub, S.E., and L.J. Trainor. 1998. Singing to infants: Lullabies and play songs. Advances in Infancy Research 12: 43–77.Google Scholar
  109. Trevarthen, C. 1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication, ed. M. Bullowa, 321–347. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  110. Trevarthen, C. 2002. Origins of musical identity: evidence from infancy for musical social awareness. In Musical identities, ed. R.A.R. MacDonald, D.J. Hargreaves, and D. Miell, 21–38. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  111. Trevarthen, C. 1993. The self born in intersubjectivity: An infant communicating. In The Perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Sources of Self-Knowledge, ed. U. Neisser 121–173. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  112. Trevarthen, C., and P. Hubley. 1978. Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In Action, gesture and symbol, ed. A. Lock, 183–229. London: Academic.Google Scholar
  113. Trevarthen, C., and V. Reddy. 2007. Consciousness in infants. In A companion to consciousness, ed. M. Velman and S. Schneider, 41–57. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  114. Tronick, E. 1989. Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist 44: 112–119.Google Scholar
  115. Tronick, E. 2007. Infant moods and the chronicity of depressive symptoms: The co-creation of unique ways of being together for good or ill. Paper 1: The normal process of development and the formation of moods. In The neurobehavioral and social-emotional development of infants and children, ed. Tronick, E 348-362. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
  116. Tronick, E.Z. 1998. Interactions that effect change in psychotherapy: a model based on infant research. Infant Mental Health Journal 19: 277–279.Google Scholar
  117. Tronick, E.Z. 2002. A model of infant mood states and Sandarian affective waves. Psychoanalytic Dialogues 12: 73–99.Google Scholar
  118. Tronick, E.Z., H. Als, and L. Adamson, 1979. Structure of early face-to-face communicative interactions. In Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication, ed. M. Bullowa, 349–370. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  119. Tronick, E.Z., H. Als, L. Adamson, S. Wise, T.B. and Brazelton, 1978. The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 17: 1–13.Google Scholar
  120. Tronick, E., and C. Reck. 2009. Infants of depressed mothers. Harvard Review of Psychiatry 17(2): 147–156.Google Scholar
  121. Tronick, E.Z., N. Bruschweiler-Stern, A.M. Harrison, K. Lyons-Ruth, A.C. Morgan, J.P. Nahum, L. Sander, et al. 1998. Dyadically expanded states of consciousness and the process of therapeutic change. Infant Mental Health Journal 19(3): 290–299.Google Scholar
  122. Vygotsky, L. 1997. The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky, Volume 4: The History of the Development of Higher Mental Functions. (M. J. Hall, Trans.). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  123. Werner, K.W., and J.J. Gross. 2009. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A conceptual framework. In Emotion regulation and psychopathology, ed. A. Kring and D. Sloan. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  124. Wexler, B. 2008. Brain and culture: Neurobiology, ideology, and social change. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  125. Wyllie, M. 2005. Lived time and psychopathology. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology 12: 173–185.Google Scholar
  126. Zelazo, P.D., and U. Müller. 2010. Executive function in typical and atypical children. In Blackwell handbook of cognitive development, 2 revisedth ed, ed. U. Goswami, 574–603. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© US Government 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyUniversity of MemphisMemphisUSA
  2. 2.Department of PhilosophyDurham UniversityDurhamUK

Personalised recommendations