Doing Good Leads to More Good: The Reinforcing Power of a Moral Self-Concept
- 866 Downloads
- 17 Citations
Abstract
What is the role of self-concept in motivating moral behavior? On one account, when people are primed to perceive themselves as “do-gooders”, conscious access to this positive self-concept will reinforce good behavior. On an alternative account, when people are reminded that they have done their “good deed for the day”, they will feel licensed to behave worse. In the current study, when participants were asked to recall their own good deeds (positive self-concept), their subsequent charitable donations were nearly twice that of participants who recalled bad deeds, or recent conversation topics, consistent with an account of moral reinforcement. In addition, among participants reporting good deeds, those who did not note whether they were recognized or unrecognized by other people donated significantly more than participants who took note of others’ responses. In sum, when people are primed to see themselves as good people, who do good for goodness’ sake, not to obtain public credit, they may be motivated to do more good.
Keywords
Moral Behavior Actual Donation Charitable Donation Good Deed Moral ReinforcementNotes
Acknowledgments
We thank Daryl Cameron, Mike Norton, Dan Ariely, Chen-Bo Zhong, Doug Medin, Sonya Sachdeva, Joshua Knobe, and Rebecca Saxe for helpful discussion and comments. This project was made possible through the support of a grant from the John Templeton Foundation.
Supplementary material
References
- Ariely, D., and M.I. Norton. 2008. How actions create–not just reveal–preferences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12(1): 13–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Barclay, P., and R. Willer. 2007. Partner choice creates competitive altruism in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Science 274(1610): 749–753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bargh, J.A., M. Chen, and L. Burrows. 1996. Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71(2): 230–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bartlett, M.Y., and D. DeSteno. 2006. Gratitude and prosocial behavior: Helping when it costs you. Psychological Science 17(4): 319–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bartlett, M.Y., P. Condon, J. Cruz, J. Baumann, and D. Desteno. 2012. Gratitude: Prompting behaviours that build relationships. Cognition & emotion 26(1): 2–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Beaman, A., M. Preston, B. Klentz, and N. Steblay. 1983. Fifteen years of foot-in-the door research: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 9: 181–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Carlson, E.N., S. Vazire, and R.M. Furr. 2011. Meta-insight: Do people really know how others see them? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101(4): 831–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Czopp, A.M., M.J. Monteith, and A.Y. Mark. 2006. Standing up for a change: Reducing bias through interpersonal confrontation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90(5): 784–803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- DeSteno, D., M.Y. Bartlett, J. Baumann, L.A. Williams, and L. Dickens. 2010. Gratitude as moral sentiment: Emotion-guided cooperation in economic exchange. Emotion 10(2): 289–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dunn, E.W., L.B. Aknin, and M.I. Norton. 2008. Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science 319(5870): 1687–1688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Epley, N., and T. Gilovich. 1999. Just going along: Nonconscious priming and conformity to social pressure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 35: 578–589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fehr, E., and S. Gachter. 2002. Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415(6868): 137–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gino, F., M.I. Norton, and D. Ariely. 2010. The counterfeit self: The deceptive costs of faking it. Psychological Science 21(5): 712–720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Greene, J.D., and J.M. Paxton. 2009. Patterns of neural activity associated with honest and dishonest moral decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(30): 12506–12511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Macrae, N., and L. Johnston. 1998. Help, I need somebody: Automatic action and inaction. Social Cognition 16: 400–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Mazar, N., and C.B. Zhong, In Press. Do green products make us better people? Psychological Science.Google Scholar
- Mazar, N., O. Amir, and D. Ariely. 2008. The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. American Marketing Association XLV: 633–644.Google Scholar
- Miller, R., P. Brickman, and D. Bolen. 1975. Attribution versus persuasion as a means of modifying behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31: 430–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Moll, J., F. Krueger, R. Zahn, M. Pardini, R. de Oliveira-Souza, and J. Grafman. 2006. Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103(42): 15623–15628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Monin, B., and D.T. Miller. 2001. Moral credentials and the expression of prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81(1): 33–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nelson, L., and M. Norton. 2005. From student to superhero: Situational primes shape future helping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 41: 423–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Rand, D.G., A. Dreber, T. Ellingsen, D. Fudenberg, and M.A. Nowak. 2009. Positive interactions promote public cooperation. Science 325(5945): 1272–1275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Sachdeva, S., R. Iliev, and D.L. Medin. 2009. Sinning saints and saintly sinners: The paradox of moral self-regulation. Psychological Science 20(4): 523–528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Shariff, A.F., and A. Norenzayan. 2007. God is watching you: Priming God concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science 18(9): 803–809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Shih, M., T.L. Pittinsky, and N. Ambady. 1999. Stereotype susceptibility: Identify salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science 10(1): 80–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stone, J., and J. Cooper. 2001. A self-standards model of cognitive dissonance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 37: 228–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Valdesolo, P., and D. DeSteno. 2007. Moral hypocrisy: Social groups and the flexibility of virtue. Psychological Science 18(8): 689–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Valdesolo, P., and D. DeSteno. 2008. The duality of virtue: Deconstructing the moral hypocrite. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44: 1334–1338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Vazire, S. 2010. Who knows what about a person? The self-other knowledge asymmetry (SOKA) model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 98(2): 281–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Vazire, S., and M.R. Mehl. 2008. Knowing me, knowing you: The accuracy and unique predictive validity of self-ratings and other-ratings of daily behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95(5): 1202–1216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Williams, L.A., and D. DeSteno. 2008. Pride and perseverance: The motivational role of pride. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94(6): 1007–1017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Williams, L.A., and D. DeSteno. 2009. Pride: Adaptive social emotion or seventh sin? Psychological Science 20(3): 284–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zhong, C.B., and K. Liljenquist. 2006. Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science 313(5792): 1451–1452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar