With nearly 100 million people infected, and over 2 million deaths to date and rising, the COVID-19 pandemic has had a pervasive impact on human society (Worldometer, 2021). In an effort to reduce the spread of the virus and related pressures on healthcare services, many countries around the world have implemented community-level restrictions, such as self-isolation or lockdown procedures, causing significant disruption to key aspects of people’s daily life. Furthermore, the highly contagious and invisible nature of the virus has transformed core human behaviors such as social interactions (e.g., shaking hands, hugging) into threatening and potentially deadly experiences. The uncertainty of living with this new pathogen and the ensuing isolation and restrictions to human interaction pose a severe risk to the mental health of the general population (Prout et al., 2020; WHO, 2020).

Confrontation with a major threat, such as a pandemic, has a range of negative consequences to mental health and psychosocial well-being. Evidence is already emerging that the implementation of lockdown measures is significantly impacting on mental health, with increasing presentations or exacerbation of stress, depression, anxiety, and sleep problems (Gloster et al., 2020; Lee et al., 2020; Murphy et al., 2020; Serafini et al., 2020; van Tilburg et al., 2020; Wang, Pan, et al., 2020; Wang, Zhang, et al., 2020; Wong et al., 2020). Heightened fear of COVID-19 has been associated with poor mental health indicators, including depression and anxiety (e.g., Ahorsu et al., 2020; Bitan et al., 2020; Fitzpatrick et al., 2020; Kanovsky & Halamová, 2020). While the unprecedented physical distancing measures have resulted in significant changes to people’s social lives and feelings of social safeness, research has documented that social connectedness may buffer against the negative physical and mental health impact of the pandemic, and promote resilience (Nitschke et al., 2020; Palgi et al., 2020; Saltzman et al., 2020). In fact, feeling socially safe is positively related to feeling connected to others and supported in close social relationships, is associated with increased resilience in the face of adversity, and is negatively linked to symptoms of depression and anxiety (Armstrong et al., 2020; Kelly et al., 2012). Social safeness has been proposed as an emotion regulation process in its own right that can be distinguished from positive affect and negative affect, and is a unique predictor of stress (Armstrong et al., 2020), which might act as a buffer against mental health problems. Social safeness is associated with being open and receptive to support and compassion from others (Gilbert, 2009; Kelly & Dupasquier, 2016; Seppälä et al., 2017), and has been found to be related with decreased traumatic impact of early adverse events and to mediate the link between early emotional trauma and depressive symptoms (Matos et al., 2015). Furthermore, robust evidence has demonstrated that psychological and social factors aggravated by the COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., stress, depression, loneliness) can increase vulnerability for infection after virus exposure (Cohen, 2021) and impair the immune system’s response to vaccines (Madison, et al., 2021), and may hence be relevant for susceptibility to COVID-19 and the SARS-CoV-2 vaccine immunization.

Therefore, investigating the protective factors that might mitigate the mental health effects of the COVID-19 pandemic and promote resilience during these adverse times is critical and a research priority for mental health science (Holmes et al., 2020; Vinkers et al., 2020). Compassion plays a pivotal role in emotion regulation, mental states, social relationships, and behavior (Seppälä et al., 2017), and may emerge as a key protective factor against the pervasive impact of the pandemic on mental health. The current study is part of a broader multinational longitudinal study investigating the buffering effects of compassion during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Although compassion can be variously defined (Mascaro et al., 2020), evolutionary-focused models (Gilbert, 2019, 2020) and ancient Buddhist traditions (Dalai Lama, 1995) conceptualize compassion as a prosocial motivation, defined as “the sensitivity to suffering in self and others, with a commitment to try to alleviate and prevent it” (Gilbert, 2014, p. 19). Being sensitive to and engaged with sources of distress rather than avoid, dissociate from, or deny them requires courage, especially in the case of the COVID-19 pandemic. Compassion, evolving from the mammalian care-giving systems, comes with a range of physiological and emotional regulating systems, particularly for down-regulating threat and allowing states of “rest and digest” (Brown & Brown, 2017; Carter et al., 2017; Mayseless, 2016). Hence, compassion is supported by evolved physiological (e.g., the myelinated vagus nerve, oxytocin) and psychological mechanisms (e.g., social intelligence and competencies) that underpin caring motives and behavior (Carter, 2014; Porges, 2007). Compassion emerges from the combination of an innate mammalian caring motivation and complex human cognitive competencies that have evolved over the last two million years (Dunbar, 2016a, b; Gilbert, 2009, 2019). Compassionate competencies encompass the social intelligences of knowing/mind awareness, empathic awareness, and knowing intentionality that transform basic caring motives into potentials for compassion (Dunbar, 2016a, b; Gilbert, 2019, 2020; Kirby & Gilbert, 2017).

When individuals are under stress, being cared for and supported by others has powerful physiological effects (Porges, 2007, 2017). Compassion can therefore be seen as a dynamic intra- and interpersonal process that unfolds in a social interactional context: there is the compassion we can express to others, the compassion that can be expressed to us from others, and our ability to be self-compassionate (Gilbert, 2009; Gilbert et al., 2011). These three flows of compassion are highly interactive and can influence each other (Gilbert, 2014; Gilbert et al., 2017), but they can also be independent, in that one may struggle with being compassionate towards oneself but be able to direct compassion to others (Lopez et al., 2018).

Emerging research is suggestive of the benefits of compassion for mental health and emotion regulation (e.g., MacBeth & Gumley, 2012), physiological health (e.g., Fredrickson et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2020; Klimecki et al., 2014), and interpersonal and social relationships (e.g., Crocker & Canevello, 2012; Yarnell & Neff, 2013). In particular, self-compassion has been shown to be a protective factor, increasing resilience to common mental health issues (MacBeth & Gumley, 2012; Muris & Petrocchi, 2017) and promoting well-being (Zessin et al., 2015). For example, self-compassion has been shown to moderate the relationship between stress, shame, or stigma and psychological distress (Blackie & Kocovski, 2019; Heath et al., 2018; Luo et al., 2018; Oliveira et al., 2018; Wong et al., 2016). Being compassionate to others has also been associated with reduced negativity (Miller et al., 2015) and stronger social connections (Cozolino, 2007; Crocker & Canevello, 2012). Moreover, the ability to be open to receiving compassion from others may buffer against depressive symptoms (Hermanto et al., 2016; Steindl et al., 2018). In addition to compassion offering well-being benefits, compassion can also be cultivated and enhanced through interventions such as Compassion-Focused Therapy (CFT; for patients) and Compassionate Mind Training (CMT; for the general public) (Gilbert, 2014, 2020; Gilbert & Procter, 2006) where it has been shown to diminish mental health issues (e.g., depression, anxiety, stress, self-criticism, shame) (Craig et al., 2020 and Kirby et al., 2017 for reviews; Irons & Heriot-Maitland, 2020; Matos et al, 2017).

In relation to the pandemic, self-compassion has been found to improve life satisfaction and coping (Li et al., 2021), cohabitation (Jimenez et al., 2020), and mediated the effect of the perceived COVID-19 threat on death anxiety (Kavakli et al., 2020) and depression, anxiety, and stress (Lau et al., 2020). Going beyond cross-sectional data, in an experimental study, Cheli et al. (2020) found that an online compassion-focused crisis intervention, aimed at promoting compassion for one’s own distress and developing one’s self-soothing abilities, reduced depression, anxiety, and stress in patients at high risk of psychosis during the pandemic. In support, Schnepper et al. (2020) found that a self-compassion intervention delivered during the pandemic via Smartphone increased self-compassion, decreased stress, and reduced eating in response to feeling anxious. Hence, existing compassion research in the context of the pandemic has focused primarily on one of the flows of compassion (i.e., self-compassion), even though its moderating role on the association between threat COVID-19, psychological distress, and social safeness remains to be studied. Furthermore, the relationships between the other flows of compassion, specifically compassion for others and receiving compassion from others, and perceived threat of COVID-19, psychological distress, and social safeness are yet to be empirically examined.

Additionally, the majority of the aforementioned studies examined a unidimensional construct of self-compassion using the Self-Compassion Scale (Neff, 2003). The proposed study builds on this literature by using a multidimensional measure which distinguishes the three flows of compassion (Compassion Engagement and Action Scales, CEAS; Gilbert et al., 2017). This scale also further distinguishes compassionate engagement (being sensitive and empathic to distress and motivated to engage with it rather than avoid it) from compassionate action (having the wisdom and skills to take the most appropriate action to alleviate distress). This offers an important distinction because being sensitive to distress but not knowing what actions to take can increase rather than decrease distress (Gilbert et al., 2017) and result in burnout (Ricard, 2015).

In previous studies, the flows of compassion for self, for others, and from others (as measured by the CEAS) have demonstrated their distinct qualities. For example, self-compassion (in particular) and receiving compassion from others tend to show the strongest associations and be the greatest predictors of depression, anxiety, stress, and positive affect (Gilbert et al., 2017; Lindsey, 2017; Matos et al., 2017; Steindl et al., 2018). Self-compassion and compassion from others moderate the relationship between negative appraisal of major life events and decreased psychological quality of life (Ferreira et al., 2021). Compassion for others shows weaker associations with distress (Gilbert et al., 2017). The general public report having higher rates of compassion for others, than for themselves or from others (Lindsey, 2017). However, all flows of compassion have been shown to be improved through CMT (Irons & Heriot-Maitland, 2020; Matos et al., 2017).

The current study aimed to explore the impact of perceived threat of COVID-19 and the three flows of compassion on mental health indicators and social safeness, in a global adult population across 21 countries from Europe, Middle East, North America, South America, Asia, and Oceania. In particular, this study aimed to examine cross-nationally whether self-compassion, compassion for others, and receiving compassion from others would moderate the effects of perceived threat of COVID-19 (i.e., fear and likelihood of contracting SARS-CoV-2) on symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress and feelings of social safeness. Given that previous studies have demonstrated the buffering effect of self-compassion against psychological distress (Blackie & Kocovski, 2019; Heath et al., 2018; Luo et al., 2018; Oliveira et al., 2018; Wong et al., 2016), we hypothesized that self-compassion would be a protective factor and significant moderator between perceived threat of COVID-19 and depression, anxiety, and stress. It was also hypothesized that compassion from others and to others (although to a lesser degree) would also act as protective factors moderating the impact of fears of COVID-19 on depression, anxiety, and stress. Furthermore, compassion is a predictor of social safeness (Akin & Akin, 2015; Kelly & Dupasquier, 2016); hence, it was hypothesized that compassion would act as a protective factor between perceived threat of COVID-19 and social safeness.

Methods

Participants

The research sample was gathered from 23 different countries. We excluded the data from Peru (N = 16) and Uruguay (N = 23) due to small sample size. The total sample consisted of 21 countries with 4057 participants, mean age 41.45 (SD = 14.96), with 80.8% (N = 3279) women, 18.2% (N = 739) men, 0.4% (N = 15) other, and 0.6% (N = 24) preferred not to respond. For demographic details per country, see Table 1.

Table 1 Research samples with sociodemographic information

Procedures

This study is part of a broader longitudinal multinational study on compassion, social connectedness, and trauma resilience during the COVID-19 pandemic. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences of the University of Coimbra (UC; CEDI22.04.2020) and was conducted in compliance with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments. When necessary, local national ethical approval was also obtained. The current analysis used cross-sectional data collected between mid-April 2020 and mid-May 2020, across 23 countries from Europe (UK, Portugal, Spain, Italy, France, Greece, Cyprus, Poland, Slovakia, Denmark), North America (USA, Canada), South America (Brazil, Argentina, Chile, Colombia, Mexico, Peru, Uruguay), Asia (China, Japan), Oceania (Australia), and Middle East (Saudi Arabia).

An online survey, combining existing and novel measures, was created by the research team in English and translated to 11 other languages using forward/backward procedures. In instances where a self-report questionnaire had already been validated for a particular language/country that version was selected. The surveys were hosted at the University of Coimbra institutional account in the online platform https://www.limesurvey.org/pt/, and a website was created to support the dissemination of the study across countries (https://www.fpce.uc.pt/covid19study/). The study was disseminated through social and traditional media platforms and institutional/professional emailing lists in each country, using snowball sampling. In addition, Facebook ads were used to promote participation among the general population in some countries. Before the completion of the survey, participants were informed about the aims of the study, procedures, and the voluntary and anonymous nature of participation. Confidentiality of the collected data was assured, and written informed consent was obtained before the completion of the study protocol. The survey was self-paced and about 25 min long. There was no payment for completing the survey.

Measures

The online survey collected sociodemographic information (nationality, country of residence, age, gender) and administered self-report instruments assessing perceived threat of COVID-19, compassion (for self, for others, from others), mental health (depression, anxiety, and stress), and social safeness.

The Perceived Coronavirus Risk Scale (PCRS; Kanovský & Halamová, 2020, adapted from Napper et al., 2012) is an 8-item self-report questionnaire that assesses participants’ fear of getting infected with SARS-CoV-2 in two dimensions: Fear of Contraction (affective aspect) and Likelihood of Contraction (cognitive aspect). Participants are asked to rate on a five-point Likert scale how much they agree with each sentence from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). It has one reversed item. Higher scores represent a higher perceived threat of COVID-19. In the original study, Kanovsky and Halamová (2020) reported internal consistency to be acceptable (Fear of Contraction α = 0.72; Likelihood of Contraction α = 0.71). In the present study, internal consistency was acceptable (Fear of Contraction α = 0.70; Likelihood of Contraction α = 0.70). Omega coefficient for the PTCS was 0.83, and Omega hierarchical 0.64. In our sample, the CFA results revealed that the PTCS model had a good fit to the data: χ2(519.23), df 13, CFI = 0.913, TLI = 0.880, RMSEA = 0.078 (90% CI 0.071–0.085), SRMR 0.050.

Compassionate Engagement and Action Scales (CEAS; Gilbert et al., 2017) includes three scales that assess the three flows of compassion: self-compassion (CEAS self-compassion), compassion for others (CEAS for others), and compassion received from others (CEAS from others), with 13 items each. Each scale measures different elements of compassion Engagement (6 items and 2 filler items) and Action (4 items and 1 filler item). Participants are asked to rate each item on a ten-point Likert scale, based on how frequently it occurs, from 1 (never) to 10 (always). Each scale can be analyzed in terms of the Engagement and Action components separately or as a single factor. Here, we use each of the three flows of compassion as single factor scales. In the original study, the CEAS showed good internal consistencies and temporal reliability (Gilbert et al., 2017). In the present study, internal consistency ranged between good and excellent: CEAS self-compassion Engagement α = 0.74/Action α = 0.89; CEAS for others Engagement α = 0.81/Action α = 0.88; CEAS from others Engagement α = 0.91/Action α = 0.93. Omega coefficient for CEAS self-compassion was 0.90, and Omega hierarchical 0.70. Omega coefficient for CEAS for others was 0.92, and Omega hierarchical 0.78. Omega coefficient for CEAS from others was 0.96, and Omega hierarchical 0.88. Results from the CFA showed that, in this study, the CEAS self-compassion model had a good fit to the data: χ2(957.55), df 34, CFI = 0.918, TLI = 0.892, RMSEA = 0.072 (90% CI 0.068–0.075), SRMR 0.068; the CEAS for others model had a good fit to the data: χ2(516.15), df 34, CFI = 0.956, TLI = 0.942, RMSEA = 0.059 (90% CI 0.056–0.063), SRMR 0.037; and the CEAS from others model had a good fit with the data: χ2(559.41), df 34, CFI = 0.972, TLI = 0.962, RMSEA = 0.062 (90% CI 0.058–0.065), SRMR 0.025.

Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scales (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) is a 21-item self-report instrument that measures three mood states: depression, anxiety, and stress, with seven items each. Participants are asked to rate on a four-point Likert scale how often items applied to them over the past week from 0 (did not apply to me at all) to 3 (applied to me very much, or most of the time). Higher scores represent higher severity of symptoms. Lovibond and Lovibond (1995) found the subscales internal consistency to range between excellent and good (Depression α = 0.91; Anxiety α = 0.84; Stress α = 0.90). In the present study, internal consistency also ranged from good to excellent (Depression α = 0.91, Anxiety α = 0.87, Stress α = 0.88). Omega coefficient for DASS was 0.96, and Omega hierarchical 0.82. CFA results revealed that the DASS model had a good fit to the data: χ2(2018.30), df 186, CFI = 0.943, TLI = 0.936, RMSEA = 0.061 (90% CI 0.058–0.063), SRMR 0.037.

Social Safeness and Pleasure Scale (SSPS; Gilbert et al., 2008) is an 11-item self-report measure that assesses the extent to which people usually experience their social world as safe, warm, and soothing and how connected they feel to others. Participants are asked to rate on a five-point Likert scale how often they feel as described in each sentence from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost all the time). Higher scores represent higher perceived social safeness and connectedness to others. In the original study, internal consistency was excellent (α = 0.92). In the present study, internal consistency is excellent (α = 0.94). Omega coefficient for SSPS was 0.95, and Omega hierarchical 0.86. Results from the CFA revealed that the SSPS model had a good fit to the data: χ2(1516.48), df 44, CFI = 0.931, TLI = 0.913, RMSEA = 0.071 (90% CI 0.068–0.084), SRMR 0.039.

Data Analyses

The data includes multiple dependent variables; for example, the DASS-21 scale has three subscales (depression, anxiety, and stress). Therefore, a multivariate multilevel model was chosen because it enables the performance of a single test of the joint effects of our independent variables on several dependent variables (Hox et al., 2017; Snijders & Bosker, 2012). Each of the models had three levels: measurements of dimensions of the DASS-21 were the level 1 units, the respondents were the level 2 units, and the countries were the level 3 units.

The statistical procedure for the three-dimensional DASS-21 was as follows: (1) fitting six multilevel multivariate models, each with three dependent variables (depression, anxiety, stress): (a) PCRS fear of contraction as the predictor (independent variable, IV), CEAS self-compassion as the predictor (IV), and their interaction (CEAS self-compassion being the moderator); (b) PCRS likelihood of contraction as the predictor (IV), the CEAS self-compassion as the predictor (IV), and their interaction (CEAS self-compassion being the moderator); (c) PCRS fear of contraction as the predictor (IV), CEAS compassion for others as the predictor (IV), and their interaction (CEAS compassion for others being the moderator); (d) PCRS likelihood of contraction as the predictor (IV), CEAS compassion for others as the predictor (IV), and their interaction (CEAS compassion for others being the moderator); (e) PCRS fear of contraction as the predictor (IV), CEAS compassion from others as the predictor (IV), and their interaction (CEAS compassion from others being the moderator); (f) PCRS likelihood of contraction as the predictor (IV), CEAS compassion from others as the predictor (IV), and their interaction (CEAS compassion from others being the moderator); (2) for each model, we tested the fit of three nested models with the data by two likelihood-ratio tests and information criteria AIC (Akaike information criterion) and BIC (Bayes Schwarz information criterion): (a) the first model was the multilevel model without taking into account three dimensions of the DASS-21, and having two main predictors (IVs) without the moderation; (b) the second model was the multivariate multilevel model taking into account three dimensions of the DASS-21, and having two main predictors (IVs) without the moderation; and finally (c) the third model was the multivariate multilevel model taking into account three dimensions of the DASS-21, and having two predictors (IVs) with the moderation. Our hypothesis in its strict form could have been retained if and only if (a) the second model had a better fit than the first one (taking into account the dimensions of the DASS-21 was justified—respondents provided different answers in DASS-21 different dimensions; otherwise, the use of the multivariate model would not be warranted); (b) the third model had the better fit than the second one—adding moderation should improve the fit. If not, only main effects (and no moderation) could have had an impact; (3) if the third model had the best fit, we would report and interpret its coefficient (p-values would be corrected by Bonferroni procedure to account for multiple testing); (4) otherwise, we would report coefficients of any model with the best fit; we also provided the graphical representations of effects. Since the SSPS is a unidimensional scale, the univariate multilevel model was sufficient. Two models were fitted: (a) PCRS fear of contraction as the predictor (IV), and (b) PCRS likelihood of contraction as the predictor (IV); and both models contained the same set of three moderators: compassion for self, compassion for others, and compassion from others.

For our multilevel model, sample size was calculated in R package simr (Green & MacLeod, 2016). For 21 countries, the overall sample size should be over 3000 participants, and sample size of each country should not be under 30 participants. The statistical power was set for 80%. To further assess the psychometric properties of the self-report scales used in the current study, Omega coefficients were computed and confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) with robust maximum likelihood estimator and Yuan-Bentler correction were conducted.

For statistical analyses, we used the R program version 4.0.3 (R Core Team, 2020), “lme4” package (Bates et al., 2015). The effects were displayed through “sjPlot” package (Lüdecke, 2018). As fixed effects, we entered the mean-centered PCRS subscale scores in an interaction with the mean-centered CEAS scales scores for each dimension of DASS-21, and for the SSPS. As random effects, we used intercepts for participants and countries for each dimension of DASS-21 and intercept for countries for the SSPS. For mean centering, we used “questionr” package (Barnier et al., 2017).

The R code syntax for the model is included in Supplementary Online Material 1. R2 (‘variance explained’) statistics were used to measure the effect size of the model. However, there is no consensus as to the most appropriate definition of R2 statistics in relation to mixed-effect models (Edwards et al., 2008; Jaeger et al., 2016; LaHuis et al., 2014; Nakagawa & Schielzeth, 2013). Even though several methods for estimating the coefficient of determination (R2) for mixed-effect models are accessible, the estimation of R2 marginal and R2 conditional in “MuMIn” package (Barton, 2015) was performed. The marginal R2 is the proportion of variability explained by the fixed effects/independent variable; the conditional R2 is the proportion of variability explained by both fixed and random effects (differences between respondents and differences between countries).

Results

Descriptive statistics of the main variables under study and Spearman correlations between the variables are reported in Table 2. Correlation results revealed that the perceived threat of COVID-19 dimensions (fear and likelihood of contraction) were positively associated with depression, anxiety, and stress, negatively linked to social safeness. Self-compassion and compassion from others were negatively correlated with depression, anxiety, and stress, and positively associated with social safeness. Compassion for others was negatively correlated with depression and positively with social safeness.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations) and Spearman’s rho correlations of the study variables (N = 4057)

In Table 3, the likelihood-ratio tests and information criteria AIC and BIC are presented. It is evident from Table 3 that all multivariate models (b-models) consistently had a better fit than models that did not take dimensionality into account. However, only models with self-compassion as moderator (1c and 2c) had a better fit than models without moderation.

Table 3 The likelihood-ratio tests and information criteria AIC and BIC for the different models

Compassion for Self

Table 4 presents coefficients of best-fitting models for self-compassion (1c, 2c). The main effects of fear of contraction on depression, anxiety, and stress were all significant (and positive). The main effects of self-compassion on all three dimensions of the DASS-21 were all significant as well (but negative). Interaction effects were significant in all three dimensions of the DASS-21 indicating that self-compassion significantly moderates the impact of the fear of contraction on depression, anxiety, and stress, across all countries. The variability among respondents was lowest in anxiety, and so was the variability among countries, which was in general larger than the individual variability, especially in depression and stress.

Table 4 Coefficients of the best-fitting models for self-compassion

Figure 1 displays marginal effects of moderation of self-compassion in the case of fear of contraction: all slopes for highly self-compassionate subjects (green) were less steep than other slopes; therefore, self-compassion buffers against the impact of fear of contraction on depression, anxiety, and stress, with the largest effect of moderation (the least parallel lines) being for anxiety, followed by stress and depression. A similar pattern was present when the likelihood of contraction was the predictor (IV), but the main effects were weaker. Self-compassion significantly moderated the impact of the likelihood of contraction on anxiety and stress (across all countries), but not depression.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Marginal effects of moderation of self-compassion (CEASselfC) on the impact of fear of contraction of COVID-19 (PTCSfearC) on depression, anxiety, and stress (DASS)

Compassion for Others

Table 5 presents coefficients of best-fitting models for compassion for others (3b, 4b). The main effects of fear of contraction on depression, anxiety, and stress were again all significant (and positive), but the main effect of compassion for others was significant (and negative) only in depression. Interaction effects were not tested, since the model with them did not significantly improve the fit with the data (see Table 3). The variability among respondents was again lowest in anxiety, and so was the variability among countries, which was larger than the individual variability, in both depression and stress. An identical pattern was discernible for the likelihood of contraction.

Table 5 Coefficients of the best-fitting models for compassion for others

Compassion from Others

Table 6 presents the coefficients of best-fitting models for compassion from others (5b, 6b). The main effects of fear of contraction and likelihood of contraction on depression, anxiety, and stress were all significant (and positive), and so were all main effects of compassion from others (but negative). Interaction effects were not tested, since the model with them did not significantly improve the fit with the data (see Table 3). The variability among respondents was lowest in anxiety, and so was the variability among countries, which was larger than the individual variability, both in depression and stress. Again, the likelihood of contraction as the predictor (IV) showed a similar pattern of results, and thus the same conclusion can be reached.

Table 6 Coefficients of best-fitting models for compassion from others

Social Safeness

Table 7 presents the coefficients of two models with the SSPS social safeness. The main effect of fear of contraction on SSPS was significant (and negative), and the main effects for self-compassion and compassion from others were significant (and positive). The main effect for compassion for others was found to be non-significant. Only compassion from others significantly moderated the effect of fear of contraction on the SSPS across all countries. While the same pattern of main effects can be seen when the likelihood of contraction is the predictor (IV), no moderation effect was found.

Table 7 Coefficients of the two models related to social safeness (SPSS)

Discussion

The aims of this study were to assess how different flows of compassion (for self, to others, from others) act as a protective factor against perceived threat of COVID-19 on mental health and social safeness. In line with our hypothesis, perceived threat of COVID-19 predicted higher scores in depression, anxiety, and stress. So, being afraid of contracting the virus was linked to increased psychological distress. This is consistent with previous studies which have revealed that fears of COVID-19 are associated to mental health difficulties (e.g., Ahorsu et al., 2020; Bitan et al., 2020; Fitzpatrick et al., 2020; Kanovsky & Halamová, 2020; Matos et al., 2021). Given that previous studies have demonstrated the buffering effect of self-compassion against psychological distress (Blackie & Kocovski, 2019; Heath et al., 2018; Luo et al., 2018; Oliveira et al., 2018; Wong et al., 2016), including in the context of COVID-19 (Jimenez et al., 2020; Kavakli et al., 2020; Lau et al., 2020; Li et al., 2021), it was hypothesized that self-compassion would be a protective factor and significant moderator between the perceived threat of COVID-19 (i.e., fear and likelihood of contraction) and depression, anxiety, and stress. This hypothesis was supported and self-compassion was found to significantly predict lower psychological distress in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. This means that individuals who were able to be compassionate towards themselves presented fewer symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress. Importantly, self-compassion moderated the impact of fear of contracting COVID-19 on depression, anxiety, and stress, acting as a protective factor. Furthermore, self-compassion buffered the effects of the perceived likelihood of contraction on anxiety and stress. This moderator effect of self-compassion was particularly strong between perceived threat of COVID-19 and anxiety. This means that, in those individuals who were able to be more compassionate towards themselves and use self-compassion as a way of coping with this threatening pandemic context, the impact of fears of COVID-19 on symptoms of anxiety, depression, and stress was less severe. In the early stages of the pandemic, these individuals were hence protected against the harmful effects of the perceived threat of COVID-19 on their mental health. This moderator effect was consistent across all 21 countries and was not affected by differences in questionnaire responses between countries.

Unique to this study was the multidimensional measurement of compassion which considers the flows of self-compassion, and compassion for others and from others. It was hypothesized that the flows of compassion for others and from others (although to a lesser degree than self-compassion) would also act as protective factors moderating the impact of perceived threat of COVID-19 on depression, anxiety, and stress. These hypotheses were not supported. While compassion for others was significantly linked to lower depressive symptoms and compassion from others was significantly associated with depression, anxiety, and stress across all countries, these flows of compassion were not significant moderators and therefore cannot be said to be protective factors against the impact of perceived threat of COVID-19 on developing or exacerbating symptoms of depression, anxiety, or stress. Previous studies using the three flows of compassion have found that compassion for others has the weakest association with other psychological distress variables (e.g., Kirby et al., 2019). The data were collected during the early stages of the pandemic and therefore fears of contracting and spreading COVID-19 and the lockdown measures implemented may have resulted in lower availability of social contact and therefore it is not surprising that compassion for others and receiving compassion from others did not emerge as significant protective factors against perceived threat of COVID-19 on mental health indicators. Interestingly, in a related study where we examined the moderator effects of fears of compassion on the context of the pandemic, the three flows of fears of compassion magnified the impact of perceived threat of COVID-19 on mental health indicators (Matos et al., 2021). A possible explanation is that, if the threat system is activated (e.g., by the pandemic), the inhibitors of compassion are likely to operate across the three flows, whereas the capacity to be compassionate to others and to receive compassion from others may have specifically been affected by fears of contraction and lockdown measures.

A second aim of this study was to consider the impact of perceived threat of COVID-19 on social safeness, as well as the moderating role of compassion. Previous research found that social connectedness can buffer against the negative physical and mental health impact of the coronavirus pandemic, and promote resilience (Nitschke et al., 2020; Palgi et al., 2020; Saltzman et al., 2020); however, the effect perceived threat of COVID-19 might have on one’s sense of social safeness has not been explored to date. We hypothesized that perceived threat of COVID-19 would have a negative relationship with social safeness, and this hypothesis was supported by our findings. So, being afraid of contracting SARS-CoV-2 was related to feeling less socially safe and connected to others. Furthermore, given that compassion is a motivation and competency which evolved from mammalian caring and is highly associated with social safeness (Akin & Akin, 2015; Kelly & Dupasquier, 2016), it was hypothesized that compassion would act as a protective factor between perceived threat of COVID-19 and social safeness. This hypothesis was partially supported. Self-compassion was significantly associated with social safeness but did not moderate the impact of fear or likelihood of contracting COVID-19 on social safeness. That is, individuals who were able to be compassionate towards themselves in the early stages of the pandemic felt more socially safe and connected to others in the context of the pandemic, but this ability to be self-compassionate did not mitigate the negative impact that the threat of contracting the virus had on their experiences of social safeness. It is possible that, given the nature of the threat of the COVID-19 pandemic, with its associated social restrictions and where others might also represent threats (virus carriers), and appear threatening (face masks), being compassionate towards oneself isn’t sufficient to mitigate the effect of the threat of the virus on feeling socially safe. In fact, our findings are in line with a prior related study exploring fears of compassion in the context of threat of COVID-19, which found that fears of self-compassion significantly predicted diminished social safeness in the context of COVID-19, but did not moderate the impact of perceived threat of COVID-19 on social safeness (Matos et al., 2021).

However, in line with our hypothesis, receiving compassion from others did emerge as a significant moderator, buffering the negative impact of fear of contracting COVID-19 on social safeness and connectedness to others. Thus, individuals who reported receiving more compassion from other people have higher feelings of social safeness and connection to others and these remained high regardless of their fear of COVID-19. Conversely, if individuals were less able to access compassion from others, then the more they perceived COVID-19 as threatening the less socially safe they would feel. This buffering effect was consistent across all 21 countries and again was not affected by individual differences between countries. These findings are supported by the same previous study where, although the three flows of fears of compassion were significant predictors of social safeness, only fears of receiving compassion from others moderated the impact of perceived threat of COVID-19 on feelings of social safeness (Matos et al., 2021). Our results are thus in line with extensive literature concerning the importance of having access to caring relationships and receiving compassion from others in regulating affective processes, physiological states and producing greater well-being and a sense of feeling connected to others and socially safe in the world (Brown & Brown, 2017; Cacioppo et al., 2008; Gilbert, 2020).

Contrarily to our initial hypothesis, compassion for others was not significantly associated with social safeness and did not emerge as a significant moderator on the association between perceived threat of COVID-19 and social safeness. It is possible that in the pandemic there would have been fewer opportunities to express compassion for others and previous research has indeed found that compassion for others has not been strongly associated with psychosocial variables (Kirby et al., 2019).

Given that self-compassion seems to buffer the potential effects of perceived threat of COVID-19 on psychological distress and given the ability of compassion from others to support social safeness in the context of fears of COVID-19, it would seem that compassion-based interventions and dissemination of compassionate strategies of public communication could be implemented to protect against mental health difficulties during and following the pandemic. Individual and group compassion-based interventions, in particular CFT (for patients) or CMT (for public) (Gilbert, 2014, 2020), cultivate compassion across the three flows including self-compassion and receiving compassion from others, and are widely evidenced to reduce psychological distress in a range of conditions and populations (Craig et al., 2020; Kirby et al., 2017; Leaviss & Uttley, 2015, for reviews). Therefore, providing greater access to individual and/or group CFT and CMT, including via Telehealth, might be pertinent. In fact, in the specific context of the pandemic, an online compassion-focused intervention was found to reduce depression, anxiety, and stress in patients at high risk of psychosis (Cheli et al., 2020). Moreover, graded online compassion-focused interventions, including psycho-education and information sharing, guided practices and strategies, and behavioral applications could be offered more widely to benefit public mental health. Social safeness and receiving compassion might have been affected by lockdown and social distancing procedures, it is therefore important to consider how compassion interventions may be helpful despite lockdown procedures. Online interventions, particularly interactive interventions with opportunities for group or partner work, could offer a good solution to delivering interventions during lockdown procedures. In addition, activities could place emphasis on social acceptance of (shared) suffering as part of the human condition, or cultivating a sense of compassion for others in the absence of direct contact (e.g., with imagery and visualization exercises). Given the evidence indicating that psychological interventions that reliably reduce anxiety and depression may boost immune responses to vaccines (Madison et al., 2021; Vedhara et al., 2019), compassion-based interventions such as CMT/CFT may be worthy of further investigation as possible adjuvants to the SARS-CoV-2 vaccine.

In addition, and while not the focus of the current study, given the acute experiences of threat and trauma reported by COVID-19 survivors (e.g., Tingey et al., 2020; Tu et al., 2021) and the damaging lasting effects of the disease on their mental health (e.g., depression, anxiety, post-traumatic stress; Liu et al., 2021; Taquet et al., 2021), it might be relevant to consider the value of compassion-based interventions for these individuals to prevent and address their mental health difficulties and promote their psychosocial well-being. In fact, mounting research has supported the positive effects of compassion-focused interventions for people with chronic or physical illness (e.g., Carvalho et al., 2021; Gooding et al., 2020).

The implementation of community-based strategies to support resilience during the COVID-19 pandemic is an important goal (Serafini et al., 2020). The current findings highlight that self-compassion and compassion from others may mitigate the psychological impact of the ongoing and long-term threat induced by the COVID-19 pandemic. Knowledge accumulated in recent research, coupled with the current study, needs to be integrated by authorities and policy makers who should rapidly adopt compassionate focused strategies, such as compassionate social marketing and public health communications, to reduce the mental health consequences of this pandemic. One such example is the Campaign to End Loneliness BeMoreUs movement in the UK (https://bemoreus.org.uk/). This movement used billboards and a website prompting ideas for how to pay more attention to and connect with others (e.g., phone a friend, talk with a stranger, volunteer in the community). Compassion-focused interventions and strategies should particularly focus on cultivating compassion towards oneself and openness to receiving compassion from others, perhaps by developing abilities to be sensitive to and tolerant of distress in oneself, and competencies to compassionate action to prevent or alleviate it, as well as being receptive to care, support, and help from others.

Limitations and Future Directions

Differences across the 21 countries in terms of rates of COVID-19 and the timing of peaks of infection and associated lockdown measures may have affected variables such as the perceived threat of COVID-19 and the amount of participants’ social contact. Additionally, previous studies have reported cross-cultural differences in the compassion flows (e.g., Steindl et al., 2020) and it is possible that the type of strategies implemented by different countries to limit the spread of the virus (e.g., hard vs mild lockdown) in the early stages of the pandemic might have influenced the interplay between compassion flows, threat of COVID-19, and psychosocial distress. Nevertheless, a key strength of the current study was the multivariate multilevel methodology used and the consistency of the predictive and moderator effects across all 21 countries, which were not dominated by individual differences between countries in the levels of the compassion flows, perceived threat of COVID-19, mental health symptoms, and social safeness, thus supporting the universality of compassion as a protective factor against mental health problems and lack of social safeness. Another limitation is that the study did not have representation from all continents. Researchers from Africa were invited but were unable to participate. Future studies should more rigorously pursue participation from less represented continents and regions. This study had an unequal gender distribution, with more female respondents. Even though no gender differences have been reported in the CEAS self-compassion and compassion from others scales, women were found to score higher than men in compassion for others (Gilbert et al., 2017). Thus, in the future research should attempt to recruit more men. Additionally, mounting research has established that many individuals can develop fears, blocks, and resistances to compassion (for self, for others, and from others) and that these increase vulnerability to mental health problems (Gilbert et al., 2011; Kirby et al., 2019 for a review). Hence, given the current findings, future studies could investigate the role of fears of compassion as potential magnifying factors of the impact of perceived threat of COVID-19 on mental health and social safeness. Finally, the cross-sectional nature of the study prevents the establishment of causality. This study is part of a broader project that is collecting longitudinal data and aims to prospectively investigate the buffering effects of compassion throughout the pandemic. Another way to establish causality would be to evaluate the effects of a compassion-focused intervention on decreasing psychological distress, and/or increasing social safeness.

In conclusion, this study provides evidence for the universal protective effects of compassion, in particular self-compassion and compassion from others, against the harmful effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health and reduced social safeness. Given the damaging effects of the COVID-19 crisis on mental well-being (e.g., Gloster et al., 2020) and the anticipated second-wave mental health pandemic (Prout et al., 2020), the promotion of mental health should constitute a public health priority. Compassion-focused interventions and communications should be prioritized by public health policy makers and providers to promote resilience and address mental health problems during and following the pandemic.