Skip to main content
Log in

The Role of Mindfulness in Viewing and Making Art in Children and Adults

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Mindfulness Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

Mindfulness training has been shown to have robust attentional and cognitive benefits. However, little is known about its effects on viewing and making art. Here, we explored the effects of mindfulness-based manipulation in art viewing and art making in two studies.

Methods

In Study 1, elementary school children (N = 59) participated in an art tour of the Kidspace gallery at the Massachusetts Museum of Contemporary Art (MASS MoCA), and viewed and made artworks either with or without mindfulness manipulation. In Study 2, university students (N = 193) were randomly assigned to either the mindfulness or the control condition, and also viewed and made artworks.

Results

In Study 1, elementary students who received mindfulness induction (vs. control) reported a larger difference in excitement levels between previously seen versus new artworks, expressing more excitement about the old vs. new artworks. Further, the artworks created by children in the mindfulness (vs. control) condition were rated by independent judges as more creative and more complex. In Study 2, university students who received the mindfulness (vs. control) induction reported better memory for previously seen vs. new artworks. Their own artworks were rated by independent judges as more creative, abstract, and expressive compared to the participants in the control condition.

Conclusions

Together, results suggest that mindfulness-based practices may result in a deeper art viewing experience, and in personal art that is more creative and expressive in both children and adults.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baas, M., Nevicka, B., & Ten Velden, F. S. (2014). Specific mindfulness skills differentially predict creative performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(9), 1092–1106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brieber, D., Nadal, M., & Leder, H. (2015). In the white cube: museum context enhances the valuation and memory of art. Acta Psychologica, 154, 36–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, J. A., Mallik, S., Babuscio, T. A., Nich, C., Johnson, H. E., Deleone, C. M., ... & Carroll, K. M. (2011). Mindfulness training for smoking cessation: results from a randomized controlled trial. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 119(1–2), 72–80.

  • Brown, K. W., Goodman, R. J., Ryan, R. M., & Anālayo, B. (2016). Correction: Mindfulness enhances episodic memory performance: evidence from a multimethod investigation. Plos One, 11(7).

  • Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 211–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chatterjee, A. (2003). Prospects for a cognitive neuroscience of visual aesthetics. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 4, 55–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colzato, L. S., Ozturk, A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Meditate to create: the impact of focused-attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3.

  • Ding, X., Tang, Y., Deng, Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2015). Mood and personality predict improvement in creativity due to meditation training. Learning and Individual Differences, 37, 217–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, X., Tang, Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2014). Improving creativity performance by short-term meditation. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 10(1), 9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eberth, J., & Sedlmeier, P. (2012). The effects of mindfulness meditation: a meta-analysis. Mindfulness, 3(3), 174–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eidelman, S., & Crandall, C. S. (2012). Bias in favor of the status quo. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(3), 270–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K. C., Nijeboer, S., Dixon, M. L., Floman, J. L., Ellamil, M., Rumak, S. P., et al. (2014). Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 43, 48–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., Reiner, K., & Meiran, N. (2012). “Mind the trap”: mindfulness practice reduces cognitive rigidity. PLoS One, 7(5), e36206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. P., Kisida, B., & Bowen, D. (2014). Value of field trips. Taking students to an arts museum improves critical thinking skills, and more. Education Next, 14, 79–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, N. R., & Clark, D. P. (2016). The observing facet of trait mindfulness predicts frequency of aesthetic experiences evoked by the arts. Mindfulness, 7(4), 971–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heeren, A., Van Broeck, N., & Philippot, P. (2009). The effects of mindfulness on executive processes and autobiographical memory specificity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(5), 403–409.

  • Jedrczak, E., Beresford, M., & Clements, G. (1985). The TM-Sidhi program, pure consciousness, creativity and intelligence. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 19, 270–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kisida, B., Bowen, D. H., & Greene, J. P. (2016). Measuring critical thinking: results from an art museum field trip experiment. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 9(1), 171–182.

  • Lebuda, I., Zabelina, D. L., & Karwowski, M. (2015). Mind full of ideas: a meta-analysis of the mindfulness–creativity link. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 22–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, D. (2004). A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 489–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locher, P., Overbeeke, K., & Wensveen, S. (2010). Aesthetic interaction: a framework. Design Issues, 26, 70–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lykins, E. L. B., Baer, R. A., & Gottlob, L. R. (2012). Performance-based tests of attention and memory in long-term mindfulness meditators and demographically matched nonmeditators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(1), 103–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., & Kassman, K. T. (2012). Stepping out of history: mindfulness improves insight problem solving. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(2), 1031–1036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Picard, D., & Gauthier, C. (2012). The development of expressive drawing abilities during childhood and into adolescence. Child Development Research, 2012, 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ren, J., Huang, Z., Luo, J., Wei, G., Ying, X., Ding, Z., ... & Luo, F. (2011). Meditation promotes insightful problem-solving by keeping people in a mindful and alert conscious state. Science China Life Sciences, 54(10), 961–965.

  • Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 92–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmertz, S. K., Anderson, P. L., & Robins, D. L. (2009). The relation between self-report mindfulness and performance on tasks of sustained attention. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31, 60–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., & Kunze, S. (2012). The psychological effects of meditation: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1139–1171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(3), 373–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Francis, A. D., Nieuwenhuis, S., Davis, J. M., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biology, 5(6), e138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., Brown, K., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Cooperation between the default mode network and the frontal–parietal network in the production of an internal train of thought. Brain Research, 1428, 60–70.

  • Specker, E., Tinio, P. P., & van Elk, M. (2017). Do you see what I see? An investigation of the aesthetic experience in the laboratory and museum. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(3), 265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, L. D., & Tobin, A. (2018). ArtInSight: a contemplative approach to museum gallery teaching and learning. Journal of Museum Education, 43(4), 334–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinio, P. P. (2013). From artistic creation to aesthetic reception: The mirror model of art. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 265–275.

  • Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(3), 136–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zabelina, D. L., Robinson, M. D., Ostafin, B. D., & Council, J. R. (2011). Manipulating mindfulness benefits creative elaboration at high levels of neuroticism. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 29(2), 243–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research was supported in part by the Mind & Life Institute, Francisco J. Varela Grant, 002869-00001A to DLZ.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DLZ: designed the study, performed data analysis, and wrote the paper. RAW: Executed the study and wrote part of the Introduction, Methods, and the Discussion sections. AT and LT designed the mindfulness manipulation, assisted in data collection, and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Darya L. Zabelina.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethics Statement

These studies have been approved by the University of Arkansas ethics committee and have therefore been performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

Informed Consent Statement

All participants gave their informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zabelina, D.L., White, R.A., Tobin, A. et al. The Role of Mindfulness in Viewing and Making Art in Children and Adults. Mindfulness 11, 2604–2612 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01474-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01474-8

Keywords

Navigation