Advertisement

Mindfulness

, Volume 9, Issue 4, pp 1117–1122 | Cite as

Meditation Inhibits Aggressive Responses to Provocations

  • David DeSteno
  • Daniel Lim
  • Fred Duong
  • Paul Condon
ORIGINAL PAPER

Abstract

Meditation has been advocated as a mental practice designed to reduce suffering and increase virtuous behavior. Although it has been previously linked to altruistic acts, its ability to reduce aggression and related retributive behaviors remains open to question. Here, we report on an experiment in which participants were randomly assigned to a mindfulness meditation or active control condition 3 weeks prior to facing a real-time provocation known to evoke aggression. Participants’ capacities for executive control were also assessed subsequent to training. Results showed that 3 weeks of daily meditation practice substantially reduced aggressive behavior even in the absence of any enhanced executive control capabilities. These results suggest that meditation attenuates aggression through direct reductions in motives to cause harm to others.

Keywords

Meditation Aggression Executive control 

Notes

Authors’ Contributions

DD, DL, FD, and PC designed the study. DL and FD executed the study. DD conducted the data analysis in collaboration with DL and FD. DD wrote the manuscript which was edited by DL, FD, and PC.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Ethical Standards

Ethics approval for the conduct of this experiment was granted by the Northeastern University Institutional Review Board to ensure that procedures were in accord with the standards noted in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. To ensure that individuals taking part in the study were participating voluntarily and that they understood their rights as well as the presence of any potential risks, all participants were offered the opportunity to ask questions prior to signing a statement of informed consent and beginning the experiment.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Use of the Headspace app was made freely available by Headspace, Inc. for this research project. Headspace did not provide any other financial support and none of the authors have any ties, financial or otherwise, to Headspace.

References

  1. Ashar, Y. K., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Yarkoni, T., Sills, J., Halifax, J., Dimidjian, S., & Wager, T. D. (2016). Effects of compassion meditation on a psychological model of charitable donation. Emotion, 16, 691–705.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Condon, P., Desbordes, G., Miller, W. B., & DeSteno, D. (2013). Meditation increases compassionate responses to suffering. Psychological Science, 24, 2125–2127.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Condon, P., & DeSteno, D. (2017). Enhancing compassion: social psychological perspectives. In E. Seppala, J. Doty, M. Worline, D. Cameron, S. Brown, & E. Simon-Thomas (Eds.), Oxford handbook of compassion science. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Condon, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Compassion for one reduces punishment for another. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(3), 698–701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Denson, T. F., Capper, M. M., Oaten, M., Friese, M., & Schofield, T. P. (2011). Self-control training decreases aggression in response to provocation in aggressive individuals. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(2), 252–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. DeSteno, D. (2015). Compassion and altruism: how our minds determine who is worthy of help. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 80–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. DeSteno, D., Valdesolo, P., & Bartlett, M. Y. (2006). Jealousy and the threatened self: getting to the heart of the green-eyed monster. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 626–641.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Finkel, E. J. (2014). The I3 model: metatheory, theory, and evidence. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 1–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Finkel, E. J., DeWall, C. N., Slotter, E. B., McNulty, J. K., Pond Jr, R. S., & Atkins, D. C. (2012). Using I3 theory to clarify when dispositional aggressiveness predicts intimate partner violence perpetration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 533.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Fix, R. L., & Fix, S. T. (2013). The effects of mindfulness-based treatments for aggression: a critical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 18, 219–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gethin, R. (1998). The foundations of Buddhism. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Kruschke, J. K. (2013). Baysesian estimation supersedes the t test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 573–603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Leiberg, S., Klimecki, O., & Singer, T. (2011). Short-term compassion training increases prosocial behavior in a newly developed prosocial game. PLoS One, 6, e17798.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Lieberman, J. D., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & McGregor, H. A. (1999). A hot new way to measure aggression: hot sauce allocation. Aggressive Behavior, 25(5), 331–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Lim, D., Condon, P., & DeSteno, D. (2015). Mindfulness and compassion: an examination of mechanism and scalability. PLoS One, 10(2), e0118221.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: effects of meditative expertise. PLoS One, 3(3), e1897.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Memedovic, S., Grisham, J. R., Denson, T. F., & Moulds, M. L. (2010). The effects of trait reappraisal and suppression on anger and blood pressure in response to provocation. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(4), 540–543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Teper, R., & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Meditation, mindfulness and executive control: the importance of emotional acceptance and brain-based performance monitoring. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(1), 85–92.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Weng, H. Y., Fox, A. S., Shackman, A. J., Stodola, D. E., Caldwell, J. Z. K., et al. (2013). Compassion training alters altruism and neural responses to suffering. Psychological Science, 24, 1171–1180.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Northeastern UniversityBostonUSA
  2. 2.Southern Oregon UniversityAshlandUSA

Personalised recommendations