Skip to main content
Log in

Longitudinal Exploration of Mindreading and Self-Conscious Emotions in Early Adolescents

  • Research in Progress
  • Published:
Psychological Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The tendency to engage in self-evaluative emotions such as guilt, shame and to blame others for social transgressions increases during early adolescence and involves mentalization skills. However, few studies explore how such emotions connect to mind reading and emotion recognition skills (i.e., theory of mind (ToM)). Accordingly, this study explored individual differences and associations among young adolescents’ ToM and shame/guilt and blame tendencies over time. This longitudinal study included Years 1–3 (2015–7) data obtained from 6th, 7th and 8th graders Canadian students (N = 146; 86 females; Mage = 11.92 years, SD = .48). Self-report questionnaires and interviews measured ToM and self-conscious emotions. Results showed no gender differences in ToM, although girls scored higher than boys on guilt across all 3 years. Boys scored higher than girls on blaming others in Year 2 and Year 3. Concurrent associations between self-affect and theory of mind were only significant at Year 1, such that girls with higher ToM skills reported higher feelings of guilt than boys. Moreover, higher levels of affective ToM were related to lower levels of blaming others and higher levels of shame in Y1 only. Implications for developmental social cognitive theory and practice are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

We’re sorry, something doesn't seem to be working properly.

Please try refreshing the page. If that doesn't work, please contact support so we can address the problem.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

References

  • Andrews, K., & Talwar, V. (2021). Considering morally relevant theory of mind through a social domain theory perspective. Journal of Moral Education. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2021.1938521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B.O. (2018). SRMR in Mplus. Retrieved on March 20th 2020, from https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/SRMR-in-Mplus-Asparouhov-Muth%C3%A9n/7d1ade06295314ea4fce371d62577c9057096280.

  • Bambrah, V., Cameron, D., & Inzlicht, M. (2022). Outrage fatigue? Cognitive costs and decisions to blame. Motivation and Emotion.

  • Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The friendship questionnaire: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 33(5), 509–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Banerjee, R., & Henderson, L. (2001). Social–cognitive factors in childhood social anxiety: A preliminary investigation. Social Development, 10(4), 558–572. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banerjee, R., & Watling, D. (2005). Children’s understanding of faux pas: Associations with peer relations. Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 2(1), 27–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batanova, M. D., & Loukas, A. (2011). Social anxiety and aggression in early adolescents: Examining the moderating roles of empathic concern and perspective taking. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1534–1543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Białecka-Pikul, M., Stępień-Nycz, M., Szpak, M., Grygiel, P., Bosacki, S. L., Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2021). Theory of mind and peer attachment in adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 31(4), 1202–1217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bigelow, L. (2022). Mindfulness meditation programs informed by transgender youth. Mindfulness. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-022-02048-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S. J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 187–207. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowker, J. C. (2011). Examining two types of best friendship dissolution during early adolescence. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 31(5), 656–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caputi, M., & Schoenborn, H. (2018). Theory of mind and internalizing symptoms during middle childhood and early adolescence: The mediating role of coping strategies. Cogent Psychology, 5, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlo, G., & Pierotti, S.(2020). The development of pro-social motives. In L. A. Jensen (Ed.), Oxford handbook of moral development: An interdisciplinary perspective. (pp. 27–51). Oxford University Press.

  • Christner, N., & Paulus, M. (2022, June). The role of the moral self-concept and moral emotions for prosocial behavior in preadolescence, Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the International Society for Behavioral Development.

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being adolescent: Conflict and growth in the teenage years. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devine, R. T. (2021). Individual differences in theory of mind in middle childhood and adolescence. In R. T. Devine & S. Lecce (Eds.), Theory of mind in middle childhood and adolescence: Integrating mutiple perspectives (pp. 55–76). Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Devine, R., & Apperly, I. (2021). Willing and able? Theory of mind, social motivation, and social competence in middle childhood and early adolescence. Developmental Science, e13137.

  • Doey, L., Coplan, R. J., & Kingsbury, M. (2014). Bashful boys and coy girls: A review of gender differences in childhood shyness. Sex Roles, 70, 255–266. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-013-0317-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagley, A. H., & Wood, W. (2016). Social role theory of sex differences. In N. Naples, R. C. Hoogland, M. Wickramasinghe, & W. C. A. Wong (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell encyclopedia of gender and sexuality studies (pp. 1–3). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

  • Else-Quest, N. M., Higgins, A., Allison, C., & Morton, L. C. (2012). Gender differences in self-conscious emotional experience: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(5), 947–981. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027930.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Furukawa, E., Tangney, J., & Higashibara, F. (2012). Cross-cultural continuities and discontinuities in shame, guilt, and pride: A study of children residing in Japan, Korea and the USA. Self and Identity, 11, 90–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, W. L., Pickett, C. L., & Brewer, M. B. (2000). Social exclusion and selective memory: How the need to belong influences memory for social events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(4), 486–496. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200266007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gazelle, H., Lundin, J., & Bosacki, S. (2022). Theory of mind, gender, gains in friendships versus peer acceptance and anxious solitude from middle childhood through early adolescence. Social Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, P., & Irons, C. (2009). Shame, self-criticism, and self-compassion in adolescence. In N. B. Allen & L. B. Sheeber (Eds.), Adolescent Emotional Development and the Emergence of Depressive Disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 195–214). Cambridge University Press.

  • Guglielmo, S., Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2009). At the heart of morality lies folk psychology. Inquiry, 52(5), 449–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A., & Montoya, A. (2017). A tutorial on testing, visualizing, and probing an interaction involving a multicategorical variable in linear regression analysis. Communication Methods and Measures, 11(1), 1–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2016.1271116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. (1975). Sex differences in moral internalisation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 720–729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kudinova, A., Brick, B., Barthelemy, C., MacPherson, H., Jenkins, G., DeYoung, L., Gilbert, A., Radoeva, P., Kim, K., Armey, M., & Dickstein, D. (2022). Sex and age moderate the trajectory of guilt among children and adolescents with and without recent suicidal ideation. Cognition and Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2022.2029359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhnert, R.-L., Begeer, S., Fink, E., & de Rosnay, M. (2017). Gender-differentiated effects of theory of mind, emotion understanding, and social preference on prosocial behavior development: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 154, 13–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S., Mezulis, A. H., & Abramson, L. Y. (2008). The ABCs of depression: Integrating affective, biological, and cognitive models to explain the emergence of the gender difference in depression. Psychological Review, 115(2), 291–313. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson Nolaker, E., Murray, K., Happé, F., & Charlton, R. A. (2018). Cognitive and affective associations with an ecologically valid test of theory of mind across the lifespan. Neuropsychology, 32(6), 754. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000464.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1971). Attribution in social interaction. General Learning Press.

  • Lagattuta, K. H., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). The development of self-conscious emotions: Cognitive processes and social influences. In J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 91–113). Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lane, J., & Bowman, L. (2022). How children’s social tendencies shape their theory of mind development: Access and attention to social information. Developmental Review, 61, 100977.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lecce, S., Caputi, M., & Pagnin, A. (2014). Long-term effect of theory of mind on school achievement: The role of sensitivity to criticism. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 305–318. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2013.821944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loades, M. E., Rimes, K., Lievesley, K., Ali, S., & Chalder, T. (2019). Cognitive and behavioural responses to symptoms in adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: A case-control study nested within a cohort. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(3), 564–579. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104519835583.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • López-Pérez, B., Gummerum, M., Jiménez, M., & Tamir, M. (2022). What do I want to feel? Emotion goals in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Child Development, 00, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., & Monroe, A. E. (2014). A theory of blame. Psychological Inquiry, 25(2), 147–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malti, T. (2016). Toward an integrated clinical-developmental model of guilt. Developmental Review, 39, 16–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McHale, S. M., Kim, J. Y., Dotterer, A. M., Crouter, A. C., & Booth, A. (2009). The development of gendered interests and personality qualities from middle childhood through adolescence: A biosocial analysis. Child Development, 80(2), 482–495. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01273.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Misailidi, P. (2018). Individual differences in children’s understanding of guilt: Links with Theory of Mind. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 179, 219–229. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2018.1474848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Misailidi, P., & Kapsali, K. (2020). Second-order theory of mind as a predictor of children’s guilt proneness. Journal of Moral Education. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2020.1854205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morken, I., Røysamb, E., Nilsen, W., & Karevold, E. (2018). Body dissatisfaction and depressive symptoms on the threshold to adolescence: Examining gender differences in depressive symptoms and the impact of social support. Journal of Early Adolescence. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431618791280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mizokawa, A. (2014). Theory of mind and sensitivity to teacher and peer criticism among Japanese children. Infant and Child Development, 24, 189–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murrar, S., Isenberg, N., Niedenthal, P., & Brauer, M. (2019). Shame and guilt among ice hockey players in the penalty box. Motivation and Emotion, 43(6), 940–947. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-019-09786-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (2018). Mplus 8.1. [Computer Software]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén.

  • Nelson, E. E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E. B., & Pine, D. S. (2005). The social re-orientation of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35(2), 163–174. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291704003915.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nikolic, M., Hannigan, L., Krebs, G., Sterne, A., Gregory, A., Eley, T., et al. (2022). Aetiology of shame and its association with adolescent depression and anxiety: Results from a prospective twin and sibling study. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 63, 99–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nikolić, M., van der Storm, L., Colonnesi, C., Brummelman, E., Kan, K. J., & Bögels, S. (2019). Are socially anxious children poor or advanced mindreaders? Child Development, 90(4), 1424–1441. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13248.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Norwich, B., Moore, D., Stentiford, L., & Hall, D. (2022). A critical consideration of ‘mental health and wellbeing’ in education: Thinking about school aims in terms of wellbeing. British Educational Research Journal, 00, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osterhaus, C., & Bosacki, S. (2022). Systematic review of advanced. Theory of Mind measures and social consequences. Developmental Review, 64, 101201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterhaus, C., & Koerber, S. (2021). The development of advanced theory of mind in middle childhood: A longitudinal study from age 5 to 10 years. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13627.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paquette, J. A., & Underwood, M. K. (1999). Gender differences in young adolescents’ experiences of peer victimization: Social and physical aggression. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly, 45(2), 242–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pizarro, D. A., & Tannenbaum, D. (2012). Bringing character back: How the motivation to evaluate character influences judgments of moral blame. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), The social psychology of morality: Exploring the causes of good and evil (pp. 91–108). American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Reicher, S., Spears, R., & Haslam, S. A. (2010). The social identity approach in social psychology. In The SAGE handbook of identities (pp. 45–62). SAGE Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446200889.n4.

  • Roos, S., Hodges, E., & Salmivalli, C. (2013). Do guilt- and shame-proneness differentially predict prosocial, aggressive, and withdrawn behaviors during early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 50, 941–946.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, K. M., Kim, H., Tolan, P. H., & Jennings, P. A. (2019). An exploration of normative social and emotional skill growth trajectories during adolescence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 62, 102–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2019.02.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, K.H., Bukowski, W.M., & Bowker, J.C. (2015). Children in peer groups. In R. Lerner (Series Ed.), M.H. Bornstein & T. Leventhal (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, Vol. 4: Ecological Settings and Processes in Developmental Systems (pp. 175–222).

  • Rudolph, K. D. (2021). Chapter Three - Understanding peer relationships during childhood and adolescence through the lens of social motivation. Advances in Motivation Science, 8, 105–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.adms.2020.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, K. D., & Flynn, M. (2007). Childhood adversity and youth depression: Influence of gender and pubertal status. Development and Psychopathology, 19(2), 497–521. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407070241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, K. D., Miernicki, M. E., Troop-Gordon, W., Davis, M. M., & Telzer, E. H. (2016). Adding insult to injury: Neural sensitivity to social exclusion is associated with internalizing symptoms in chronically peer- victimized girls. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuro-Science, 11(5), 829–842. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultzberg, M., & Muthén, B. (2018). Number of subjects and time points needed for multilevel time-series analysis: A simulation study of dynamic structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 25(4), 495–515. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2017.1392862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schipper, M., Goagoses, N., & Koglin, U. (2022). Associations between moral identity, social goal orientations, and moral decisions in adolescents. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2022.2056160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding: Developmental and clinical analyses. New York, USA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2008). Recognition of “Fortune of others” emotions in asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1451–1461. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0515-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skymba, H., Joyce, C., Telzer, E., & Rudolph, K. (2022). Peer adversity predicts interpersonal needs in adolescent girls. Journal of Research on Adolescence. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12741.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Silfver, M., & Helkama, K. (2007). Empathy, guilt, and gender: A comparison of two measures of guilt. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48, 239–246. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2007.00578.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S., Dutcher, K., Aksar, M., Talwar, V., & Bosacki, S. (2018). Emotional competencies in emerging adolescence: Relations between teacher ratings and student self-reports. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2018.1455059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soland, J., Rimm-Kaufman, S., Kuhfield, M., & Venture-Abbas, N. (2022). Empirical benchmarks for changes in social and emotional skills over time. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13741.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Somerville, L. H. (2013). The teenage brain: Sensitivity to social evaluation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 121–127. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413476512.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Song, J.-H., Volling, B. L., Lane, J. D., & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Aggression, sibling antagonism, and theory of mind during the first year of siblinghood: A developmental cascade model. Child Development, 87(4), 1250–1263.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Slepian, M. L., & Bastian, B. (2017). Truth or punishment: Secrecy and punishing the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43, 1595–1611. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217717245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stearns, P. (2016). Shame, and a challenge for emotions history. Emotion Review, 8(3), 197–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, H. (1953). The Interpersonal Theory of Psychiatry. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P. (1991). Moral affect: The good, the bad, and the ugly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 598–607.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271–324. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Dearing, R. L., Wagner, P. E., & Gramzow, R. (2000). The test of self- conscious affect-3 (TOSCA-3). George Mason University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P. E., & Gramzow, R. (1989). The test of self-conscious affect (TOSCA). George Mason University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Martinez, A. G. (2014). Two faces of shame: The role of shame. Psychological Science, 25, 799–805.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, D., Uhlmann, E. L., & Diermeier, D. (2011). Moral signals, public outrage, and immaterial harms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(6), 1249–1254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, E. F., & McGarty, C. A. (2009). The role of efficacy and moral outrage norms in creating the potential for international development activism through group-based interaction. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48(1), 115–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treeby, M., Prado, C., Rice, S., & Crowe, S. (2016). Shame, guilt, and facial emotion processing: Initial evidence for a positive relationship between guilt-proneness and facial emotion recognition ability. Cognition and Emotion, 30(8), 1504–1511. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1072497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Lissa, C., Hawk, S., Koot, H., Branje, S., & Meeus, W. (2017). The cost of empathy: Parent–adolescent conflict predicts emotion dysregulation for highlyempathic youth. Developmental Psychology, 53(9), 1722.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vásquez-Rosati, A., Montefusco-Siegmund, R., López, V., & Cosmelli, D. (2019). Emotional influences on cognitive flexibility depend on individual differences: A combined micro-phenomenological and psychophysiological study. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01138.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vellante, M., Baron-Cohen, S., Melis, M., Marrone, M., Petretto, D., Masala, C., & Preti, A. (2013). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test: Systematic review of psychometric properties and a validation study in Italy. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 18(4), 326–354. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2012.721728.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Venter, E., & Uys, H. (2018). Group therapy for early adolescent relationship problems between girls. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2018.1509794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voracek, M., & Dressler, S. G. (2006). Lack of correlation between digit ratio (2D:4D) and Baron- Cohen’s “Reading the mind in the eyes” test, empathy, systemising, and autism-spectrum quotients in a general population sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(8), 1481–1491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, S. (2005). Gender differences in the relationship between young children’s peer-related social competence and individual differences in theory of mind. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166, 297–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weimer, A. A., Warnell, K. R., Ettekal, I., Cartwright, K. B., Guajardo, N. R., & Liew, J. (2021). Correlates and antecedents of theory of mind development during middle childhood and adolescence An integrated model. Developmental Review, 59, 100945.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodhouse, S. S., Dykas, M. J., & Cassidy, J. (2012). Loneliness and peer relations in adolescence. Social Development, 21, 273–293. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00611.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research was supported in part by SSRHC Insight Grant #435–2015-0010.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SB and VT were responsible for conceptualization and manuscript writing. MC analyzed the data and edited the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sandra Bosacki.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Ethics Approval

Ethics clearance was obtained from the University Research Ethics Board (REB # 58–0615).

Consent to Participate

Written informed consent was obtained from all participants in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bosacki, S., Talwar, V. & Castellanos, M. Longitudinal Exploration of Mindreading and Self-Conscious Emotions in Early Adolescents. Psychol Stud 68, 342–358 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-023-00727-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-023-00727-5

Keywords

Navigation