Abstract
Keepon is a small creature-like robot designed for simple, natural, nonverbal interaction with children. The minimal design of Keepon’s appearance and behavior is meant to intuitively and comfortably convey the robot’s expressions of attention and emotion. For the past few years, we have been observing interactions between Keepon and children at various levels of physical, mental, and social development. With typically developing children, we have observed varying styles of play that suggest a progression in ontological understanding of the robot. With children suffering from developmental disorders such as autism, we have observed interactive behaviors that suggest Keepon’s design is effective in eliciting a motivation to share mental states. Finally, in developing technology for interpersonal coordination and interactional synchrony, we have observed an important role of rhythm in establishing engagement between people and robots. This paper presents a comprehensive survey of work done with Keepon to date.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
American Psychiatric Association (1994) DSM-IV: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association, Arlington
Baird G, Simonoff E, Pickles A, Chandler S, Loucas T, Meldrum D, Charman T (2006) Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: The Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet 368:210–215
Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge
Bernieri FJ, Rosenthal R (1991) Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In: Feldman RS, Rime B (eds) Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 401–432
Billard A (2002) Play, dreams and imitation in Robota. In: Dautenhahn K et al. (eds) Socially intelligent agent. Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht, pp 165–173
Breazeal C, Scassellati B (2000) Infant-like social interactions between a robot and a human caretaker. Adapt Behav 8:49–74
Burgoon JK, Stern LA, Dillman L (1995) Interpersonal adaptation: Dyadic interaction patterns. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Butterworth G, Jarrett N (1991) What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. Br J Dev Psychol 9:55–72
Chapple ED (1982) Movement and sound: The musical language of body rhythms in interaction. In: Davis M (ed) Interaction rhythms: Periodicity in communicative behavior. Human Sciences Press, New York, pp 31–51
Condon WS, Ogston WD (1966) Sound film analysis of normal and pathological behavior patterns. J Nerv Ment Dis 143:338–347
Condon WS, Ogston WD (1967) A segmentation of behavior. J Psychiatr Res 5:221–235
Condon WS, Ogston WD (1971) Speech and body motion synchrony or the speaker-hearer. In: Horton DL, Jenkins JJ (eds) Perception of language. Charles E Merrill, Columbus, pp 150–173
Condon WS, Sander LW (1974) Synchrony demonstrated between movements of the neonate and adult speech. Child Dev 45(2):456–462
Condon WS, Sander LW (1974) Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science 183(4120):99–101
Cruz RF, Berrol CF (2004) Dance/movement therapists in action: A working guide to research options. Charles C. Thomas, Springfield
Dalton KM, Nacewicz BM, Johnstone T, Schaefer HS, Gernsbacher MA, Goldsmith HH, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ (2005) Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat Neurosci 8:519–526
Dautenhahn K (1997) I could be you: The phenomenological dimension of social understanding. Cybern Syst J 28:417–453
Dautenhahn K (1999) Robots as social actors: Aurora and the case of autism. In: Proceedings of the international cognitive technology conference, pp 359-374
DeCasper AJ, Fifer WP (1980) Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science 208:1174–1176
Dennett DC (1987) The intentional stance. MIT Press, Cambridge
Fantz RL (1961) The origin of form perception. Sci Am 204:66–72
Fernald A, Mazzie C (1991) Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Dev Psychol 27:209–221
Fledderjohn H, Sewickley J (1993) An annotated bibliography of dance/movement therapy, 1940–1990. American Dance Therapy Association, Columbia
Frith U (1989) Autism: Explaining the enigma. Blackwell, London
Kaye K (1982) The mental and social life of babies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Kendon A (1970) Movement coordination in social interaction: Some examples described. Acta Psychol 32:100–125
Kendon A (1990) Conducting interaction: Patterns of behavior in focused encounters. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Kozima H (2002) Infanoid: A babybot that explores the social environment. In: Dautenhahn X et al. (eds) Socially intelligent agent. Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht, pp 157–164
Kozima H, Ito A (2003) From joint attention to language acquisition. In: Leather J, van Dam J (eds) Ecology of language acquisition. Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht, pp 65–81
Kozima H, Nakagawa C, Yano H (2004) Can a robot empathize with people? Int J Artif Life Robot 8:83–88
Kozima H, Nakagawa C (2006) Interactive robots as facilitators of children’s social development. In: Mobile robots: Toward new applications, pp 269–286
Kozima H, Nakagawa C, Yasuda Y (2007) Children-robot interaction: A pilot study in autism therapy. Prog Brain Res 164:385–400
Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198:75–78
Michalowski MP, Sabanovic S, Kozima H (2007) A dancing robot for rhythmic social interaction. In: Proceedings of the 2nd annual conference on human-robot interaction (HRI 2007), pp 89–96
Michalowski MP, Kozima H (2007) Methodological issues in facilitating rhythmic play with robots. In: Proceedings of the 16th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication (RO-MAN 2007)
Michaud F, Théberge-Turmel C (2002) Mobile robotic toys and autism. In: Dautenhahn K et al. (eds) Socially intelligent agent. Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht, pp 125–132
Morton J, Johnson MH (1991) Conspec and conlearn: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychol Rev 98:164–181
Nadel J, Butterworth G (1999) Imitation in infancy. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Okada M, Goan M (2005) Modeling sociable artificial creatures: Findings from the Muu project. In: Proceedings of the 13th international conference on perception and action
Robins B, Dickerson P, Stribling P, Dautenhahn K (2004) Robot-mediated joint attention in children with autism: A case study in robot-human interaction. Interact Stud 5:161–198
Scassellati B (2005) Using social robots to study abnormal social development. In: Proceedings of the 5th international workshop on epigenetic robotics, pp 11–14
Scheirer E (1998) Tempo and beat analysis of acoustic music signals. J Acoust Soc Am 103(1):588–601
Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28(5):675–691
Trevarthen C (2001) Intrinsic motives for companionship in understanding: Their origin, development, and significance for infant mental health. Infant Ment Health J 22:95–131
Trevarthen C, Malloch SN (2000) The dance of wellbeing: Defining the musical therapeutic effect. Nord J Music Ther 9(2):3–17
Vygotsky LS (1934/1962) Thought and language. MIT Press, Cambridge
World Health Organization (1993) The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioral disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. World Health Organization, Geneva
Zlatev J (2001) The epigenesis of meaning in human beings, and possibly in robots. Minds Mach 11:155–195
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Kozima, H., Michalowski, M.P. & Nakagawa, C. Keepon. Int J of Soc Robotics 1, 3–18 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-008-0009-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-008-0009-8