Skip to main content

Biological Pedagogy as Concern for Semiotic Growth

Abstract

Deweyan pedagogy seeks to promotes growth, characterized as an increased sensitivity, responsiveness, and ability to participate in an environment. Growth, Dewey says, is fostered by the development of habits that enable further habit formation. Unfortunately, humans have their own habitual ways of encountering other species, which often do not support growth. In this article, I briefly review some common conceptions of learning and the process of habit-formation to scope out the landscape of a more responsible and responsive approach to taking growth seriously. What emerges is a reflexive biosemiotics that has humans explicitly concerned with the in situ emergence of new signification in themselves and in other organisms. This requires we take a pedagogical stance in our attitudes and practices towards other species, which we can enrich with insights derived from re-interpreting traditional empirical studies. By freeing the habit-forming process from confining stereotype, a biological pedagogy can enable a more fluid and creative biosphere, unencumbered to explore unfolding possibilities in semiotic space.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

We’re sorry, something doesn't seem to be working properly.

Please try refreshing the page. If that doesn't work, please contact support so we can address the problem.

Notes

  1. Hoffmeyer writes: “Whenever a regular behavior or habit of an individual or species is interpreted as a sign by some other individuals (conspecific or alter-specific) and is reacted upon through the release of yet other regular behaviors or habits, we have a case of semethic interaction” (p. 15, 2008b)

  2. Kotov and Kull (2011) describe a Lotman-inspired semiosphere as a the “set of relations comprising everything living” (p. 191) on the planet. However, just as “biosphere” often unnecessarily draws us away from our active participation in and effect on the world, “semiosphere” bears the same risks as a global, abstract category. Since the purpose of biological pedagogy includes increasing the capacity of humans to respond with semiotic sensitivity to their interspecies relations, a regional term is preferable. Of course, any semioregion is connected by some semiotic transaction to some other, ultimately unifying as a semiosphere. But it would trivialize our embeddedness and responsibility to equalize semiotic relations in this way. Finally, semioregion should not be thought of in terms of geographical proximity but in terms of semiotic interactivity.

  3. Other approaches, such as cognitive ethology (Allen and Beckoff 1997) will not be discussed for two reasons: they have not captured the public imagination as much as the three approaches identified, and their primary aim is to provide descriptions and explanations of biological phenomena (and are therefore subject to many of the concerns raised in this paper).

  4. While rightly maligned by subject-centered biologists, if humans paid better attention to behaviour, they would still develop deeper relationships and sensitivity towards other species than is the case with “everyday” nativism. Behaviorism can dislodge our habit of thinking of other species in terms of “built-in mechanisms” that leads many people to ignore the subtle shades of differentiation and change in the biotic world around them.

  5. Such thinking could serve equally well to keep someone solipsistically detached from even their closest family and friends.

  6. Nevertheless, it is also certain that once we take seriously our call to engage with other species as educators, we will not continue to conduct science the way we have been. I don’t imagine that much of the manipulations of the experimental method will make much sense when embarking in pedagogical-minded experimentation. This is the topic of another paper.

References

  • Abram, D. (1985). The perceptual implications of Gaia. The Ecologist, 15(3), 96–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abram, D. (1996). The spell of the sensuous: Perception and language in a more-than-human world. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Affifi, R. (2013). Learning plants: semiosis between the part and the whole. Biosemiotics. doi:10.1007/s12304-013-9164-x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, C., & Beckoff, M. (1997). Species of mind: The philosophy and biology of cognitive ethology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (2003). The double life of B.F. Skinner: inner conflict, dissociation and the scientific taboo against consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(1), 5–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, C. A. (1993). Education, cultural myths, and the ecological crisis: Toward deep changes. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G. M. (1991). Cognitive ethology and critical anthropomorphism: A snake with two heads and hognose snakes that play dead. In C. A. Ristau (Ed.), Cognitive ethology: The minds of other animals: Essays in honor of Donald R. Griffin (pp. 53–90). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1916). Democracy and education: And introduction to the philosophy of education. New York: MacMillan Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1922). Human nature and conduct. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1929). Experience and nature. New York: Dover.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1938). Experience and education. Indianapolis: Kappa Delta Pi.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrison, J. (1997). Dewey and Eros: Wisdom and desire in the art of teaching. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouinlock, J. (1972). John Dewey’s philosophy of value. New York: Humanities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, D. T. (Ed.). (2006). John Dewey and our educational prospect: A critical engagement with Dewey’s democracy and education. Albany: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haraway, D. J. (2008). When species meet. Minnesota: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardie, C. D. (1962). Truth and fallacy in educational theory. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, P., & Peters, R. S. (1970). The logic of education. London: Routledge & Kegal Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2001). Seeing virtuality in nature. Semiotica, 134(1), 381–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2004). Uexküllian Planmässigkeit. Sign Systems Studies, 32(1), 73–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2008a). Biosemiotics: An examination into the signs of life and the life of signs. Scranton: University of Scranton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2008b). The semiotic niche. Journal of Mediterranean Ecology, 9, 5–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmeyer, J. (2010). A biosemiotic approach to the question of meaning. Zygon, 45(2), 367–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohl, H. (1984). Growing minds: On becoming a teacher. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotov, K., & Kull, K. (2011). Semiosphere is the relational biosphere. In C. Emmeche & K. Kull (Eds.), Towards a semiotic biology: Life is the action of signs (pp. 179–194). London: Imperial College Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Maran, T. (2006). Where do your borders lie? Reflections on the semiotical ethics of nature. In C. Gersdorf & S. Mayer (Eds.), Nature in literary and cultural studies: Translatlantic conversations on ecocriticism. Amsterdam: Rodopi B.V.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noddings, N. (1998). Philosophy of education. Boulder: Westview.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nyberg, D. (1975). Pruning the growth metaphor. The Educational Forum, 40(1), 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee, J., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrilli, S., & Ponzio, A. (2005). Semiotics unbounded: Interpretive routes through the open network of signs. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smuts, B. (2001). Encounters with animal minds. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5–7), 293–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smuts, B. (2006). Between species: science and subjectivity. Configurations, 14(1–2), 115–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stjernfelt, F. (2012). The evolution of semiotic self-control: Sign evolution as the ongoing refinement of the basic argument structure of biological metabolism. In T. Schillhab, F. Stjernfelt, & T. Deacon (Eds.), The symbolic species evolved (pp. 39–64). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tønnessen, M. (2003). Umwelt ethics. Sign Systems Studies, 31(1), 281–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Uexküll, J. (2010). A foray into the worlds of animals and humans: With a theory of meaning. Minnesota: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weston, A. (1996). Self-validating reduction: a theory of environmental devaluation. Environmental Ethics, 18, 115–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Two anonymous reviewers provided thoughtful comments on an earlier draft of this article. I wish to thank Anthony Weston for years of scattered conversations, and for the glorious twin notions of “ethics-based epistemology” and “self-validating reduction.” This paper is deeply steeped with these concepts and would not exist without them. I also want to express my gratitude to everyone at the 12th Annual Gathering for Biosemiotics who listened to me present some earlier reflections on the themes raised in this article, and especially those who engaged with me to discuss these issues further. Myrdene Anderson and Timo Maran stood out as particularly supportive. I’d also like to thank Sophavanh Phommixay for her open-minded approach to animals, and Black, who has just edged his way up onto my lap.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ramsey Affifi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Affifi, R. Biological Pedagogy as Concern for Semiotic Growth. Biosemiotics 7, 73–88 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-013-9178-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-013-9178-4

Keywords

  • Biological pedagogy
  • Pedagogical stance
  • Intentional stance
  • Dewey
  • Growth
  • Semiotic growth
  • Interspecies education
  • Semethic interaction
  • Learning