Abstract
Circadian rhythms are considered a masterstroke of natural selection, which gradually increase the adaptability of species to the Earth’s rotation. Importantly, the nervous system plays a key role in allowing organisms to maintain circadian rhythmicity. Circadian rhythms affect multiple aspects of cognitive functions (mainly via arousal), particularly those needed for effort-intensive cognitive tasks, which require considerable top-down executive control. These include inhibitory control, working memory, task switching, and psychomotor vigilance. This mini review highlights the recent advances in cognitive functioning in the optical and multimodal neuroimaging fields; it discusses the processing of brain cognitive functions during the circadian rhythm phase and the effects of the circadian rhythm on the cognitive component of the brain and the brain circuit supporting cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
References
Refinetti R. Homeostasis and circadian rhythmicity in the control of body temperature. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1997, 813(1): 63–70
Huang R C. The discoveries of molecular mechanisms for the circadian rhythm: The 2017 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine. Biomedical Journal, 2018, 41(1): 5–8
DeMairan J. Histoire de l’Academie Royale des Sciences. Paris 1729
Reddy S, Reddy V, Sharma S. Physiology, Circadian Rhythm. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing, 2020
Burke T M, Scheer F A J L, Ronda J M, Czeisler C A, Wright K P Jr. Sleep inertia, sleep homeostatic and circadian influences on higher-order cognitive functions. Journal of Sleep Research, 2015, 24(4): 364–371
Valdez P. Circadian rhythms in attention. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 2019, 92(1): 81–92
Walker W H 2nd, Walton J C, DeVries A C, Nelson R J. Circadian rhythm disruption and mental health. Translational Psychiatry, 2020, 10(1): 28
Bennett C L, Petros T V, Johnson M, Ferraro F R. Individual differences in the influence of time of day on executive functions. American Journal of Psychology, 2008, 121(3): 349–361
Qasrawi S O, Pandi-Perumal S R, BaHammam A S. The effect of intermittent fasting during Ramadan on sleep, sleepiness, cognitive function, and circadian rhythm. Sleep and Breathing, 2017, 21(3): 577–586
Owens J A. Sleep in children: cross-cultural perspectives. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 2004, 2(3): 165–173
Preckel F, Lipnevich A, Schneider S, Roberts R D. Chronotype, cognitive abilities, and academic achievement: a meta-analytic investigation. Learning and Individual Differences, 2011, 21(5): 483–492
Ni Y, Wu L, Jiang J, Yang T, Wang Z, Ma L, Zheng L, Yang X, Wu Z, Fu Z. Late-night eating-induced physiological dysregulation and circadian misalignment are accompanied by microbial dysbiosis. Molecular Nutrition & Food Research, 2019, 63(24): e1900867
Wehrens S M T, Christou S, Isherwood C, Middleton B, Gibbs M A, Archer S N, Skene D J, Johnston J D. Meal timing regulates the human circadian system. Current Biology, 2017, 27(12): 1768–1775.e3
Shi L, Liu Y, Jiang T, Yan P, Cao F, Chen Y, Wei H, Liu J. Relationship between mental health, the CLOCK gene, and sleep quality in surgical nurses: a cross-sectional study. BioMed Research International, 2020, 4795763
Kim H Y, Seo K, Jeon H J, Lee U, Lee H. Application of functional near-infrared spectroscopy to the study of brain function in humans and animal models. Molecules and Cells, 2017, 40(8): 523–532
Huettel S A, Song A W, McCarthy G. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates, 2004
Boas D A, Brooks D H, Miller E L, DiMarzio C A, Kilmer M, Gaudette R J, Zhang Q. Imaging the body with diffuse optical tomography. IEEE Signal Processing Magazine, 2001, 18(6): 57–75
Niedermeyer E, da Silva F H L. Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2005
Anderson J R. Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications. New York: Worth Publishers, 2005
Cohen R A, Sparling-Cohen Y A, O’Donnell B F. The Neuropsychology of Attention. New York: Springer, 1993
Graw P, Kräuchi K, Knoblauch V, Wirz-Justice A, Cajochen C. Circadian and wake-dependent modulation of fastest and slowest reaction times during the psychomotor vigilance task. Physiology & Behavior, 2004, 80(5): 695–701
Lim J, Dinges D F. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 2008, 1129(1): 305–322
Folkard S. Diurnal variation in logical reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 1975, 66(1): 1–8
Cohen R A. Neural mechanisms of attention. In: Cohen R A, Sparling-Cohen Y A, O’Donnell B F, eds. The Neuropsychology of Attention. New York: Springer, 2014, 211–264
Sturm W, Willmes K. On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. NeuroImage, 2001, 14(1 Pt 2): S76–S84
Gazzaley A, Rissman J, Cooney J, Rutman A, Seibert T, Clapp W, D’Esposito M. Functional interactions between prefrontal and visual association cortex contribute to top-down modulation of visual processing. Cerebral Cortex, 2007, 17(Suppl 1): i125–i135
Morecraft R J, Geula C, Mesulam M M. Architecture of connectivity within a cingulo-fronto-parietal neurocognitive network for directed attention. Archives of Neurology, 1993, 50(3): 279–284
Soshi T, Kuriyama K, Aritake S, Enomoto M, Hida A, Tamura M, Kim Y, Mishima K. Sleep deprivation influences diurnal variation of human time perception with prefrontal activity change: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One, 2010, 5 (1): e8395
Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A, Talamantes J, Armijo P, Borrani J. Circadian rhythms in components of attention. Biological Rhythm Research, 2005, 36(1–2): 57–65
Riley M. Musical Listening in the German Enlightenment: Attention, Wonder and Astonishment. London: Taylor & Francis, 2017
Matchock R L, Mordkoff J T. Chronotype and time-of-day influences on the alerting, orienting, and executive components of attention. Experimental Brain Research, 2009, 192(2): 189–198
Nicholls C, Bruno R, Matthews A. Chronic cannabis use and ERP correlates of visual selective attention during the performance of a flanker go/nogo task. Biological Psychology, 2015, 110: 115–125
Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A, Talamantes J, Cortez J. Circadian and homeostatic variation in sustained attention. Chronobiology International, 2010, 27(2): 393–416
Johnson S W, North R A. Opioids excite dopamine neurons by hyperpolarization of local interneurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 1992, 12(2): 483–488
Carrier J, Monk T H. Circadian rhythms of performance: new trends. Chronobiology International, 2000, 17(6): 719–732
Wright K P Jr, Hull J T, Czeisler C A. Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans. American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 2002, 283(6): R1370–R1377
Bonnefond A, Rohmer O, Hoeft A, Muzet A, Tassi P. Interaction of age with time of day and mental load in different cognitive tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2003, 96(3 Pt 2): 1223–1236
Babkoff H, Caspy T, Mikulincer M, Sing H C. Monotonic and rhythmic influences: a challenge for sleep deprivation research. Psychological Bulletin, 1991, 109(3): 411–428
Sagaspe P, Sanchez-Ortuno M, Charles A, Taillard J, Valtat C, Bioulac B, Philip P. Effects of sleep deprivation on Color-Word, Emotional, and Specific Stroop interference and on self-reported anxiety. Brain and Cognition, 2006, 60(1): 76–87
Krishnan H C, Lyons L C. Synchrony and desynchrony in circadian clocks: impacts on learning and memory. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), 2015, 22(9): 426–437
Dinges D F, Pack F, Williams K, Gillen K A, Powell J W, Ott G E, Aptowicz C, Pack A I. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4–5 hours per night. Sleep, 1997, 20(4): 267–277
Gillberg M, Åkerstedt T. Sleep loss and performance: no “safe” duration of a monotonous task. Physiology & Behavior, 1998, 64 (5): 599–604
McCarthy M E, Waters W F. Decreased attentional responsivity during sleep deprivation: orienting response latency, amplitude, and habituation. Sleep, 1997, 20(2): 115–123
De Gennaro L, Ferrara M, Curcio G, Bertini M. Visual search performance across 40 h of continuous wakefulness: Measures of speed and accuracy and relation with oculomotor performance. Physiology & Behavior, 2001, 74(1–2): 197–204
Chee Y L, Crawford J C, Watson H G, Greaves M. Guidelines on the assessment of bleeding risk prior to surgery or invasive procedures. British Journal of Haematology, 2008, 140(5): 496–504
Drummond SPA, Brown G G. The effects of total sleep deprivation on cerebral responses to cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2001, 25(5 Suppl 1): S68–S73
Tomasi D, Wang R L, Telang F, Boronikolas V, Jayne M C, Wang G J, Fowler J S, Volkow N D. Impairment of attentional networks after 1 night of sleep deprivation. Cerebral Cortex, 2009, 19(1): 233–240
Chee M W L, Goh C S F, Namburi P, Parimal S, Seidl K N, Kastner S. Effects of sleep deprivation on cortical activation during directed attention in the absence and presence of visual stimuli. NeuroImage, 2011, 58(2): 595–604
De Havas J A, Parimal S, Soon C S, Chee M W. Sleep deprivation reduces default mode network connectivity and anti-correlation during rest and task performance. NeuroImage, 2012, 59(2): 1745–1751
Muto V, Jaspar M, Meyer C, Kussé C, Chellappa S L, Degueldre C, Balteau E, Shaffii-Le Bourdiec A, Luxen A, Middleton B, Archer S N, Phillips C, Collette F, Vandewalle G, Dijk D J, Maquet P. Local modulation of human brain responses by circadian rhythmicity and sleep debt. Science, 2016, 353(6300): 687–690
Miyake A, Shah P. Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999, 506
Baddeley A. The fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 1996, 93(24): 13468–13472
Awh E, Jonides J, Smith E E, Buxton R B, Frank L R, Love T, Wong E C, Gmeindl L. Rehearsal in spatial working memory: evidence from neuroimaging. Psychological Science, 1999, 10(5): 433–437
Stern C E, Sherman S J, Kirchhoff B A, Hasselmo M E. Medial temporal and prefrontal contributions to working memory tasks with novel and familiar stimuli. Hippocampus, 2001, 11(4): 337–346
McDowell S, Whyte J, D’Esposito M. Working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 1997, 35(10): 1341–1353
Gerstner J R, Yin J C P. Circadian rhythms and memory formation. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2010, 11(8): 577–588
Domagalik A, Oginska H, Beldzik E, Fafrowicz M, Pokrywka M, Chaniecki P, Marek T. Long-term reduction of short wavelength light affects sustained attention and visuospatial working memory. bioRxiv, 2019, 581314
Laird D A. Relative performance of college students as conditioned by time of day and day of week. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1925, 8(1): 50–63
Vedhara K, Hyde J, Gilchrist I D, Tytherleigh M, Plummer S. Acute stress, memory, attention and cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2000, 25(6): 535–549
Potter P, Wolf L, Boxerman S, Grayson D, Sledge J, Dunagan C, Evanoff B. Understanding the cognitive work of nursing in the acute care environment. Journal of Nursing Administration, 2005, 35(7–8): 327–335
Blake M JF. Time of day effects on performance in a range of tasks. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9(6): 349–350
Rowe G, Hasher L, Turcotte J. Age and synchrony effects in visuospatial working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2009, 62(10): 1873–1880
Folkard S. Time of day and level of processing. Memory & Cognition, 1979, 7(4): 247–252
Lewandowska K, Wachowicz B, Marek T, Oginska H, Fafrowicz M. Would you say “yes” in the evening? Time-of-day effect on response bias in four types of working memory recognition tasks. Chronobiology International, 2018, 35(1): 80–89
Chee M W L, Choo W C. Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. Journal of Neuroscience, 2004, 24(19): 4560–4567
Choo W C, Lee W W, Venkatraman V, Sheu F S, Chee M W. Dissociation of cortical regions modulated by both working memory load and sleep deprivation and by sleep deprivation alone. NeuroImage, 2005, 25(2): 579–587
Mu Q, Mishory A, Johnson K A, Nahas Z, Kozel F A, Yamanaka K, Bohning D E, George M S. Decreased brain activation during a working memory task at rested baseline is associated with vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Sleep, 2005, 28(4): 433–448
Chee M W L, Chuah L Y M, Venkatraman V, Chan W Y, Philip P, Dinges D F. Functional imaging of working memory following normal sleep and after 24 and 35 h of sleep deprivation: Correlations of fronto-parietal activation with performance. NeuroImage, 2006, 31(1): 419–428
Lim J, Choo W C, Chee M W L. Reproducibility of changes in behaviour and fMRI activation associated with sleep deprivation in a working memory task. Sleep (Basel), 2007, 30(1): 61–70
Honma M, Soshi T, Kim Y, Kuriyama K. Right prefrontal activity reflects the ability to overcome sleepiness during working memory tasks: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One, 2010, 5(9): e12923
McKenna B S, Eyler L T. Overlapping prefrontal systems involved in cognitive and emotional processing in euthymic bipolar disorder and following sleep deprivation: a review of functional neuroimaging studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 2012, 32(7): 650–663
Thomas R J, Rosen B R, Stern C E, Weiss J W, Kwong K K. Functional imaging of working memory in obstructive sleep-disordered breathing. Journal of Applied Physiology, 2005, 98(6): 2226–2234
McKenna B S, Sutherland A N, Legenkaya A P, Eyler L T. Abnormalities of brain response during encoding into verbal working memory among euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 2014, 16(3): 289–299
Drummond S P A, Walker M, Almklov E, Campos M, Anderson D E, Straus L D. Neural correlates of working memory performance in primary insomnia. Sleep (Basel), 2013, 36(9): 1307–1316
Stroop J R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 1992, 121(1): 15–23
Hartley L R, Shirley E. Color-name interference at different times of day. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1976, 61(1): 119–122
Manly T, Lewis G H, Robertson IH, Watson P C, Datta A. Coffee in the cornflakes: time-of-day as a modulator of executive response control. Neuropsychologia, 2002, 40(1): 1–6
Harrison Y, Jones K, Waterhouse J. The influence of time awake and circadian rhythm upon performance on a frontal lobe task. Neuropsychologia, 2007, 45(8): 1966–1972
Bratzke D, Steinborn M B, Rolke B, Ulrich R. Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and Simon tasks. Chronobiology International, 2012, 29(1): 55–61
Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Leclercq Y, Sterpenich V, Vandewalle G, Phillips C, Berthomier P, Berthomier C, Tinguely G, Gais S, Schabus M, Desseilles M, Dang-Vu T, Salmon E, Degueldre C, Balteau E, Luxen A, Cajochen C, Maquet P, Collette F. Circadian preference modulates the neural substrate of conflict processing across the day. PLoS One, 2012, 7(1): e29658
Miller E K, Cohen J D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 2001, 24(1): 167–202
Dove A, Pollmann S, Schubert T, Wiggins C J, von Cramon D Y. Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 2000, 9(1): 103–109
Heuer H, Kleinsorge T, Klein W, Kohlisch O. Total sleep deprivation increases the costs of shifting between simple cognitive tasks. Acta Psychologica, 2004, 117(1): 29–64
Bratzke D, Rolke B, Steinborn M B, Ulrich R. The effect of 40 h constant wakefulness on task-switching efficiency. Journal of Sleep Research, 2009, 18(2): 167–172
Shinkai S, Watanabe S, Kurokawa Y, Torii J. Salivary cortisol for monitoring circadian rhythm variation in adrenal activity during shiftwork. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 1993, 64(7): 499–502
Ramírez C, García A, Valdez P. Identification of circadian rhythms in cognitive inhibition and flexibility using a Stroop task. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 2012, 10(2): 136–144
Mednick S A. The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 1962, 69(3): 220–232
Sherman S M, Mumford J A, Schnyer D M. Hippocampal activity mediates the relationship between circadian activity rhythms and memory in older adults. Neuropsychologia, 2015, 75: 617–625
May C P. Synchrony effects in cognition: the costs and a benefit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1999, 6(1): 142–147
Wieth M B, Zacks R T. Time of day effects on problem solving: When the non-optimal is optimal. Thinking & Reasoning, 2011, 17 (4): 387–401
West R, Murphy K J, Armilio M L, Craik F I, Stuss D T. Effects of time of day on age differences in working memory. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 2002, 57(1): 3–10
Lu F M, Yuan Z. PET/SPECT molecular imaging in clinical neuroscience: recent advances in the investigation of CNS diseases. Quantitative Imaging in Medicine and Surgery, 2015, 5(3): 433–447
Chen B, Moreland J, Zhang J. Human brain functional MRI and DTI visualization with virtual reality. Quantitative Imaging in Medicine and Surgery, 2011, 1(1): 11–16
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by MYRG2019-00082-FHS and MYRG2018-00081-FHS Grants from the University of Macau, as wellasFDCT025/2015/A1 and FDCT0011/2018/A1 Grants from the Macau Government.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Shiyang Xu is a Ph.D. student at Faculty of Health Sciences, Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, China. He received the Master’s degree in Developmental and Educational Psychology from Shaanxi Normal University, China. He concentrates on the neuroimaging (EEG/fNIRS) with disease and children development.
Miriam Akioma is a full-time Ph.D. research scholar at the Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences in Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Macau, China. She holds Master’s degree in Applied Linguistics, and Specialization in Developmental Psychology. Her current research focuses on neuroimaging techniques for mapping the neural mechanisms and correlates related to language production, and comprehension in the bilingual brain. For that, she makes use of recent high-technologies such as fNIRS, EEG, fMRI, and eye-tracking.
Zhen Yuan is an associate professor with Faculty of Health Sciences (FHS) at University of Macau (UM), China. He also has the second appointment as the Interim Director of Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences at UM. His academic investigation is focused on cutting-edge research and development in laser, ultrasound and EEG/fMRI-related biomedical technologies as well as their clinical/pre-clinical applications in cognitive neuroscience and brain disorders. He has achieved national and international recognition through over 200 SCI publications in high ranked journals in his field. He is the editorial board member of Quantitative Imaging in Medicine and Surgery, editor of Journal of Innovative Optical Health Sciences, senior associate editor of BMC Medical Imaging, and associate editor of Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. He is a senior member of OSA and SPIE, and committee member of Chinese Biomedical Optical Society.
Supporting Information
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Xu, S., Akioma, M. & Yuan, Z. Relationship between circadian rhythm and brain cognitive functions. Front. Optoelectron. 14, 278–287 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12200-021-1090-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12200-021-1090-y