Abstract
Background
Diurnal preference (and chronotype more generally) has been implicated in exercise behavior, but this relation has not been examined using objective exercise measurements nor have potential psychosocial mediators been examined. Furthermore, time-of-day often moderates diurnal preference’s influence on outcomes, and it is unknown whether time-of-exercise may influence the relation between chronotype and exercise frequency.
Purpose
The current study examined whether individual differences in diurnal preference (“morningness-eveningness”) predict unique variance in exercise frequency and if commonly studied psychosocial variables mediate this relation (i.e., behavioral intentions, internal exercise control, external exercise control, and conscientiousness). Moreover, the study sought to test whether individuals’ typical time-of-exercise moderated the impact of diurnal preference on exercise frequency.
Methods
One hundred twelve healthy adults (mean age = 25.4; SD = 11.6 years) completed baseline demographics and then wore Fitbit Zips® for 4 weeks to objectively measure exercise frequency and typical time-of-exercise. At the end of the study, participants also self-reported recent exercise.
Results
Diurnal preference predicted both self-reported exercise and Fitbit-recorded exercise frequency. When evaluating mediators, only conscientiousness emerged as a partial mediator of the relation between diurnal preference and self-reported exercise. In addition, time-of-exercise moderated diurnal preference’s relation to both self-reported exercise and Fitbit-recorded exercise frequency such that diurnal preference predicted higher exercise frequency when exercise occurred at a time that was congruent with one’s diurnal preference.
Conclusion
Based on these findings, diurnal preference is valuable, above and beyond other psychological constructs, in predicting exercise frequency and represents an important variable to incorporate into interventions seeking to increase exercise.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Physical activity guidelines for Americans. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. 2008.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy people 2010: understanding and improving health. Washington, DC: U.S. Government printing office; 2000.
Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Predicting and changing behavior: The reasoned action approach. New York (NY): Psychology Press; 2010
Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hal; 1986.
Prochaska JO, Velicer WF. The transtheoretical model of health behavior change. Am J Health Promot. 1997; 12(1): 38–48.
Bogg T, Roberts BW. Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychol Bull. 2004; 130(6); 887–919.
Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis, NLD. Youth attitudes. In: Smith AL, Biddle SJH, eds. Youth physical activity and sedentary behavior: challenges and solutions. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2008: 167–192.
Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR. The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perpect Psyhcol Sci. 2007; 2(4): 313–345.
Conner M, Abraham C. Conscientiousness and the theory of planned behavior: Toward a more complete model of the antecedents of intentions and behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2001; 27(11): 1547–1561.
McEachen RRC, Sutton S, Myers LB. Mediation of personality influences on physical activity within the theory of planned behavior. J Health Psychol. 2010; 15(8): 1170–1180.
Van Dongen HP, Dinges DF. Circadian rhythms in fatigue, alertness, and performance. Prin Pract Sleep Med. 2000; 20:391–9.
Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: Daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythm. 2003; 18(1): 80–90.
Smith CS, Reilly C, Midkiff K. Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. J Appl Psychol. 1989; 74(5): 728–738.
Killgore WDS. Effect of sleep deprivation and morningness–eveningness traits of risk taking. Psychol Rep. 2007; 100(2): 613–626.
Wang L, Chartrand TL. Morningness–eveningness and risk taking. J Psychol Interdicip Appl. 2015; 149(4): 394–411.
Díaz-Morales JF. Morning and evening-types: Exploring their personality styles. Per. Individ. Dif. 2007; 43(4): 769–778.
Facer-Childs E, Brandstaetter R. The impact of circadian phenotype and time since awakening on diurnal performance in athletes. Curr Biol. 2015; 25(4): 518–522.
Thun E, Bjorvatn B, Flo E, Harris A, Pallesen S. Sleep, circadian rhythms, and athletic performance. Sleep Med Rev. 2015; 23: 1–9.
Kunorozva L, Roden LC, Rae DE. Perception of effort in morning-type cyclists is lower when exercising in the morning. J Sports Sci. 2014; 32(10): 917–925.
Brown FM, Neft EE, LaJambe CM. Collegiate rowing crew performance varies by morningness-eveningness. J Strength Cond Res. 2008; 22(6): 1894–1900.
Schaal S, Peter M, Randler C. Morningness-eveningness and exercise in adolescents. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2010; 8(2): 147–159.
Lipnevich AA, Credé M, Hahn E, Spinath FM, Roberts RD, Preckel F. How distinctive are morningness and eveningness from the Big Five-Factors of personality? A meta-analytic investigation. J Pers and Soc Psychol. In press.
Bailey KJ, Jung ME. The early bird gets the worm! Congruency between intentions and behavior is highest when plans to exercise are made for the morning. J Appl Biobehave Res. 2014; 19(4): 233–247.
May CP, Hasher L. Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1998: 24(2): 363–379.
Rowe G, Hasher L, Turcotte J. Age and synchrony effects in visuospatial working memory. Q J Exp Psychol. 2009; 62(10): 1873–1880.
Preckel F, Lipnevich AA, Boehme K, et al. Morningness-eveningness and educational outcomes: The lark has an advantage over the owl at high school. Br J Educ Psychol. 2013; 83(1): 114–134.
Tavernier R, Willoughby T. Are all evening-types doomed? Latent class analyses of perceived morningness–eveningness, sleep and psychosocial functioning among emerging adults. Chronobiol. Int. 2014; 31(2): 232–242.
Goldstein D, Hahn CS, Hasher L, Wiprzycka UJ, Zelazo PD. Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in morning versus evening type adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect? Pers Individ Dif. 2007; 42(3): 431–440.
Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009; 41, 1149–1160.
Horne JA., Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness–eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976; 4: 97–110.
Torsvall L, Akerstedt T. A diurnal type scale. Construction, consistency and validation in shift work. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1980; 6: 283–90.
Natale V, Alzani A. Additional validity evidence for the composite scale of morningness. Pers Individ Dif. 2001; 30(2): 293–301.
Jankowski KS. Composite scale of morningness: Psychometric properties, validity with Munich ChronoType Questionnaire and age/sex differences in Poland. Eur Psychiatry. 2015; 30: 166–171.
Ajzen, I. Theory of Planned Behavior Questionnaire. Measurement Instrument Database for the Social Science. Available at www.midss.org. 2013. Accessibility verified September, 28, 2016.
Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NLD, Biddle SJH. A meta-analytic review of the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior in physical activity: Predictive validity and contribution of additional variables. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2002; 24: 3–32.
Jekauc D, Völkle M, Wagner MO, Mess F, Reiner M, Renner B. Prediction of attendance at fitness center: A comparison between the theory of planned behavior, the social cognitive theory, and the physical activity maintenance theory. Front Psychol. 2015; 121(6): 1–10.
Sechrist KR, Walker SN, Pender NJ. Development and psychometric evaluation of the exercise benefits/barriers scale. Res Nurs Health. 1987; 10(6): 357–365.
Grubbs L, Carter J. The relationship of perceived benefits and barriers to reported exercise behaviors in college undergraduates. Fam Com Health. 2002; 25(2):76–84.
Goldberg LR, Johnson JA, Eber HW, et al. The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. J Res Pers. 2006; 40(1): 84–96.
Costa PT, McCrae RR. The revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PIR). In: Boyle GJ, Matthews G, Saklosfke DH, eds. The SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment. London: SAGE; 2008: 179–198.
Ainsworth BE, Haskell WL, Herrmann SD, et al. Compendium of Physical Activities: The second update of activity codes and MET intensities to classify the energy cost of human physical activities. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011; 43(8), 1575–1581.
Mammen G, Gardiner S, Senthinathan A, McClemont L, Stone M, Faulkner G. (2012). Is this bit fit? Measuring the quality of the Fitbit step-counter. Health Fit J of Can, 5(4); 30–39.
Gusmer R, Bosch T, Watkins A, Ostrem J, Dengel D. Comparison of FitBit® Ultra to ActiGraph™ GT1M for assessment of physical activity in young adults during treadmill walking. Open Sports Med J. 2014; 8: 11–15.
Takacs J, Pollock CL, Guenther JR, Bahar M, Napier C, Hunt MA. Validation of the Fitbit one activity monitor device during treadmill walking. J Sci Med Sport. 2013; 17(5):496–500.
Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjostrom M, et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2003; 195: 1381–1395.
IBM Corp. IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 22.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp; 2013.
Hayes AF. PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling. www.afhayes.com. Published 2012.
Bauer DJ, Curran PJ. Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: Inferential and graphical techniques. Multivariate Beh Res. 2005; 40(3): 373–400.
Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, et al. Social jetlag: Misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int. 2006; 23(1&2): 497–509.
Kitamura, S., Hida, A., Watanabe, M., et al. Evening preference is related to the incidence of depressive states independent of sleep-wake conditions. Chronobiol Int. 2010; 27(9–10): 1797–1812.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
Authors’ Statement of Conflict of Interest and Adherence to Ethical Standards Authors Hisler, Phillips and Krizan declare that they have no conflict of interest. All procedures, including the informed consent process, were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000.
Ethical Adherence
This research was approved by an institutional review board and involved human participants who provided informed consent to participate in the research.
About this article
Cite this article
Hisler, G.C., Phillips, A.L. & Krizan, Z. Individual Differences in Diurnal Preference and Time-of-Exercise Interact to Predict Exercise Frequency. ann. behav. med. 51, 391–401 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9862-0
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9862-0