Skip to main content
Log in

Cue-Induced Smoking Urges Deplete Cigarette Smokers’ Self-Control Resources

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Annals of Behavioral Medicine

Abstract

Background

Exposure to smoking-related cues leads to increased urge to smoke in regular cigarette smokers and resisting these urges requires considerable self-control.

Purpose

Adopting a resource depletion model, two studies tested the hypothesis that resisting smoking urges depletes self-control resources.

Methods

Adopting a within-participants randomized cross-over design, participants (study 1, N = 19; study 2, N = 32) were exposed to smoking-related (study 1: smoking images; study 2: cigarette cue-exposure task) and neutral (study 1: neutral images; study 2: drinking-straw task) cues with presentation order randomized. After each cue set, participants completed self-control tasks (study 1: handgrip task; study 2: handgrip and Stroop tasks), performance on which constituted dependent measures of self-control.

Results

Self-control task performance was significantly impaired when exposed to smoking-related cues compared to neutral cues. No significant presentation-order effects, or interaction effects between stimulus and presentation order, were found.

Conclusions

Findings corroborate our hypothesis that resisting smoking urges depletes cigarette smokers’ self-control resources and suggests that self-control capacity is governed by a limited resource.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Notes

  1. We omitted the data of one participant from the original sample (N = 20) as the data were incomplete as she opted to withdraw from the experiment early.

  2. In reality, the cigarettes in each pile were from the same brand and therefore identical in order to control for brand preference.

  3. The protocols for the cigarette and drinking-straw evaluation (cue exposure) tasks are available from the first author on request.

  4. We also tested for differences across smoking and neutral cue exposure on critical single items from the brief Questionnaire of Smoking Urges scale shown to have equal sensitivity to abstinence and reliability to the full scale. Specifically, we found significant differences on item 5 “All I want right now is a cigarette” (study 1: smoking image exposure M = 2.58, SE = 0.30, neutral image exposure M = 2.00, SE = 0.24, t(18) = 3.64, p = .002, d = 1.67; study 2: smoking cue exposure M = 3.41, SE = 0.28, neutral cue exposure, M = 2.13, SE = 0.18, t(31) = 5.57, p < .001, d = 1.97) and item 6 “I have an urge for a cigarette” (Study 1: smoking image exposure M = 3.68, SE = 0.45, neutral image exposure, M = 3.11, SE = 0.41, t(18) = 2.25, p = .037, d = 1.03; Study 2: smoking cue exposure M = 4.63, SE = 0.26, neutral cue exposure, M = 3.00, SE = 0.28, t(31) = 7.59, p < .001, d = 2.68).

  5. We adjusted the time participants spent on the handgrip task after exposure to each image set for baseline strength by subtracting baseline handgrip duration from handgrip task duration after each image set. The more negative the scores, the greater the level of self-control resource depletion

  6. We repeated the ANOVAs with Tobacco Dependence Screener scores as a covariate to control for tobacco dependence. The pattern of findings was identical to the analyses with Tobacco Dependence Screener scores excluded and there were no significant main or interaction effects for Tobacco Dependence Screener scores on the handgrip (both studies) and Stroop (study 2) dependent variables. We therefore report the ANOVA results with the Tobacco Dependence Screener scores excluded.

References

  1. Borland R. Slip-ups and relapse in attempts to quit smoking. Addictive Behaviors. 1990;15:235-245.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Chornock WM, Stitzer ML, Gross J, Leischow S. Experimental-model of smoking reexposure—Effects on relapse Psychopharmacology. 1992;108:495-500.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Shadel WG, Martino SC, Setodji C, et al. Lapse-induced surges in craving influence relapse in adult smokers: An experimental investigation. Health Psychology. 2011;30:588-596.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Shiffman S. Responses to smoking cues are relevant to smoking and relapse. Addiction. 2009;104:1617-1618.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Ferguson SG, Shiffman S. The relevance and treatment of cue-induced cravings in tobacco dependence. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2009;36:235-243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. O’Connell KA, Schwartz JE, Shiffman S. Do resisted temptations during smoking cessation deplete or augment self-control resources? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2008;22:486-495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. West R. The Multiple facets of cigarette addiction and what they mean for encouraging and helping smokers to stop. Journal of Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease. 2009;6:277-283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Muraven M, Tice DM. Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:1252-1265.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Muraven M, Tice DM, Baumeister RF. Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:774-789.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Hagger MS, Wood C, Stiff C, Chatzisarantis NLD. Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136:495-525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Muraven M, Shmueli D. The self-control costs of fighting the temptation to drink. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2006;20:154-160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Shmueli D, Prochaska JJ. Resisting tempting foods and smoking behavior: Implications from a self-control theory perspective. Health Psychology. 2009;28:300-306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Heckman BW, Ditre JW, Brandon TH. The restorative effects of smoking upon self-control resources: A negative reinforcement pathway. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012;121:244-249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Muraven M, Baumeister RF. Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:247-259.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Nordgren LF, Chou EY. The push and pull of temptation: The bidirectional influence of temptation on self-control. Psychological Science. 2011;22(11):1386-1390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Webb TL, Sniehotta FF, Michie S. Using theories of behaviour change to inform interventions for addictive behaviours. Addiction. 2010;105:1879-1892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Baxter BW, Hinson RE. Is smoking automatic? Demands of smoking behavior on attentional resources. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:59-66.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Cox LS, Tiffany ST, Christen AG. Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2001;3:7-16.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Morgan MJ, Davies GM, Willner P. The Questionnaire of Smoking Urges is sensitive to abstinence and exposure to smoking-related cues. Behavioural Pharmacology. 1999;10:619-626.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. LaRowe SD, Saladin ME, Carpenter MJ, Upadhyaya HP. Reactivity to nicotine cues over repeated cue reactivity sessions. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:2888-2899.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kawakami N, Takatsuka N, Inaba S, Shimizu H. Development of a screening questionnaire for tobacco/nicotine dependence according to ICD-10, DSM-III-r, and DSM-IV. Addictive Behaviors. 1999;24:155-166.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Gilbert DG, Rabinovitch NE. International Smoking Image Series with neutral counterparts (1.2 Ed.). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University at Carbondale; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Attwood AS, O’Sullivan H, Leonards U, Mackintosh B, Munafo MR. Attentional bias training and cue reactivity in cigarette smokers. Addiction. 2008;103:1875-1882.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Waters AJ, Shiffman S, Sayette MA, et al. Cue-provoked craving and nicotine replacement therapy in smoking cessation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:1136-1143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Sayette MA, Hufford MR. Effects of cue exposure and deprivation on cognitive resources in smokers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103:812-818.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Mayer JD, Gaschke YN. The experience and meta-experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;55:102-111.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, Tice DM. The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:351-355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Hagger MS, Wood C, Stiff C, Chatzisarantis NLD. The strength model of self-regulation failure and health-related behavior. Health Psychology Review. 2009;3:208-238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Soar K, Dawkins L, Begum H, Parrott AC. The effects of cigarette smoking and abstinence on auditory verbal learning. Human Psychopharmacology-Clinical and Experimental. 2008;23:621-627.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Hughes JR, Keenan RM, Yellin A. Effect of tobacco withdrawal on sustained attention. Addictive Behaviors. 1989;14:577-580.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Muraven M, Baumeister RF, Tice DM. Longitudinal improvement of self-regulation through practice: Building self-control strength through repeated exercise. Journal of Social Psychology. 1999;139:446-457.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Hagger MS, Chatzisarantis NLD. The sweet taste of success: The presence of glucose in the oral cavity moderates the depletion of self-control resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2013;39:27-41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Knauper B, Pillay R, Lacaille J, McCollam A, Kelso E. Replacing craving imagery with alternative pleasant imagery reduces craving intensity. Appetite. 2011;57:173-178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Hagger MS, Lonsdale A, Koka A, et al. An intervention to reduce alcohol consumption in undergraduate students using implementation intentions and mental simulations: A cross-national study. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2012;19:82-96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Erblich J, Bovbjerg DH. In vivo versus imaginal smoking cue exposures: Is seeing believing? Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2004;12:208-215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Conflict of Interest

The authors have no conflict of interest to disclose.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Martin S. Hagger B.A. (Hons)., Dip. Psych., Ph.D..

About this article

Cite this article

Hagger, M.S., Leaver, E., Esser, K. et al. Cue-Induced Smoking Urges Deplete Cigarette Smokers’ Self-Control Resources. ann. behav. med. 46, 394–400 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-013-9520-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-013-9520-8

Keywords

Navigation