Skip to main content
Log in

Beyond He and She: Does the Singular Use of “They, Them, Their” Function Generically as Inclusive Pronouns for Cisgender Men and Women?

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Gender Issues Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The American Psychological Association’s (Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, American Psychological Association, 2019) style manual recently updated its guidelines to include the use they/them/their pronouns for situations where gender is unknown or irrelevant, which includes situations involving cisgender men and women. As such, we experimentally tested whether non-binary pronouns (“they/them/their”) would function as generic and inclusive singular pronouns for cisgender men and women. As a replication and extension of previous research (i.e., Crawford and English in J Psycholinguist Res 13 (5):373–3381, https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01068152, 1984; Stout and Dasgupta in Personal Soc Psychol Bull 37 (6):757–769, https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211406434, 2011), cisgender U.S. college students (N = 381; 269 women and 112 men; M age = 19.41 years old) were randomly assigned to read a job advertisement using: (1) masculine pronouns “he/him/his”, (2) binary pronouns such as “she or he”, or (3) singular non-binary pronouns “they/them/their”. Participants’ memory for the content of the job advertisement was tested along with assessments of sexism and belongingness (i.e., ostracism or feelings of exclusion, whether they identified with the described job, and whether they would be motivated for the work). As predicted, there were gender differences in memory scores in the masculine (men scored higher) and binary (women scored higher) pronoun conditions, but not in the non-binary condition. For all three indicators of belongingness, as predicted, men’s belongingness scores were similar across the three conditions (i.e., men were included or represented by the pronouns used in all three conditions), whereas women’s scores indicated less belongingness when masculine condition pronouns were used (i.e., where women were excluded by the pronouns used) in comparison to when the binary and non-binary pronoun were used (i.e., where women were included by the pronouns used). Together these findings provide empirical support for the use of “they/them/their” as singular non-binary pronouns to refer generically and inclusively to both cisgender men and women.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Data and are not currently publicly available. Materials related to the memory test are publicly available on the Open Science Framework (see https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/NKQXU).

Code Availability

N/A.

References

  1. American Psychological Association. (1994). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (4th ed.). American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  2. American Psychological Association. (2019). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (7th ed.). American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Arthur, A. E., Bigler, R. S., Liben, L. S., Gelman, S. A., & Ruble, D. N. (2008). Gender stereotyping and prejudice in young children: A developmental intergroup perspective. In S. R. Levy & M. Killen (Eds.), Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood (pp. 66–86). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Baranowski, M. (2002). Current usage of the epicene pronoun in written English. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 6(3), 378–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88(4), 354–364. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Bem, S. L., & Bem, D. J. (1973). Does sex-biased job advertising “aid and abet” sex discrimination? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 3(1), 6–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1973.tb01290.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Bigler, R. S., & Leaper, C. (2015). Gendered language: Psychological principles, evolving practices, and inclusive policies. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(1), 187–194. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215600452

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2006). A developmental intergroup theory of social stereotypes and prejudice. In R. V. Kail & R. V. Kail (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 39–89). Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162–166. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Bradley, E. D. (2020). The influence of linguistic and social attitudes on grammaticality judgments of singular ‘they.’ Language Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2020.101272

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Crawford, M., & English, L. (1984). Generic versus specific inclusion of women in language: Effects on recall. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 13(5), 373–381. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01068152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Dahlen, S. (2021). Do we need the word ‘woman’ in healthcare? Postgraduate Medical Journal, 97(1150), 483–484. https://doi.org/10.1136/postgradmedj-2021-140193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. DeLoache, J. S., Cassidy, D. J., & Carpenter, C. J. (1987). The three bears are all boys: Mothers’ gender labeling of neutral picture book characters. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 17(3–4), 163–178. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287623

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Gastil, J. (1990). Generic pronouns and sexist language: The oxymoronic character of masculine generics. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 23, 629–643. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00289252

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Geiger, A. W., & Graf, N. (2019). About one-in-five US adults know someone who goes by a gender-neutral pronoun. Pew Research Center.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Goldberg, S. (2017). Gender revolution [special issue]. National Geographic, 231(1), 1–70.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Golden, C. R., & McHugh, M. C. (2017). The personal, political, and professional life of Sandra Bem. (2017). Sex Roles: Journal of Research, 76, 529–543. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0674-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Green, H., & Riddington, A. (2020). Gender inclusive language in perinatal services: Mission statement and rationale. https://www.bsuh.nhs.uk/maternity/wpcontent/uploads/sites/7/2021/01/Gender-inclusive-language-in-perinatal-services.pdf

  19. Hamilton, M. C. (1988). Using masculine generics: Does generic he increase male bias in the user's imagery? Sex roles, 19(11), 785–799. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00288993

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Henley, N. M. (1989). Molehill or mountain? What we know and don’t know about sex bias in language. In M. Crawford & M. Gentry (Eds.), Gender and thought: Psychological perspectives. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Hyde, J. S. (1984). Children’s understanding of sexist language. Developmental Psychology, 20(4), 697. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.4.697

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581–592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Hyde, J. S., Bigler, R. S., Joel, D., Tate, C. C., & Van Anders, S. M. (2019). The future of sex and gender in psychology: Five challenges to the gender binary. American Psychologist, 74(2), 171–193. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000307

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Keener, E. (2015). The complexity of gender: It is all that and more….in sum, it is complicated. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 73, 481–489. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-015-0542-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Lambdin, J. R., Greer, K. M., Jibotian, K. S., Wood, K. R., & Hamilton, M. C. (2003). The animal = male hypothesis: Children’s and adults’ beliefs about the sex of non-sex-specific stuffed animals. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 48(11–12), 471–482. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023567010708

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Laverne, D. (2020, September 13). 4 steps to crate train your puppy + simple secret. Retrieved November 19, 2020, from https://pawleaks.com/4-steps-to-crate-train-your-dog/

  27. Leaper, C. (2014). Gender similarities and differences in language. In T. M. Holtgraves (Ed.), Oxford library of psychology. The Oxford handbook of language and social psychology (pp. 62–81). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Leaper, C., & Bigler, R. S. (2004). Gendered language and sexist thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69(1), 128–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Liben, L. S., & Signorella, M. L. (1980). Gender-related schemata and constructive memory in children. Child Development, 51(1), 11–18. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129584

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. MacKay, D. G. (1980). Psychology, prescriptive grammar, and the pronoun problem. American Psychologist, 35(5), 444–449. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.35.5.444

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52(4), 1119–1134. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Martyna, W. (1980). Beyond the “he/man” approach: The case for nonsexist language. Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 5(3), 482–493. https://doi.org/10.1086/493733

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Maxwell, S. E., Lau, M. Y., & Howard, G. S. (2015). Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? American Psychologist, 70(6), 487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Milles, K. (2011). Feminist language planning in Sweden. Current Issues in Language Planning, 12(1), 21–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/14664208.2011.541388

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Modern Language Association of America. (2016). MLA handbook for writers of research papers (8th ed.). Modern Language Association of America.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Modern Language Association of America. (2020). How do I use singular they? Modern Language Association of America.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Moulton, J., Robinson, G. M., & Elias, C. (1978). Sex bias in language use: “Neutral” pronouns that aren’t. American Psychologist, 33(11), 1032–1036. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.33.11.1032

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Moulton, K., Han, C., Block, T., Gendron, H., & Nederveen, S. (2020). Singular they in context. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 5(1), 122. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.1012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Pollitt, K. (2015, June 29). Who has abortions? The Nation. Retrieved October 8, 2021, from https://www.thenation.com/article/archive/who-has-abortions/

  40. Signorella, M. L., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1997). A meta-analysis of children’s memories for own-sex and other-sex information. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18(3), 429–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(97)80009-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Stahlberg, D., Braun, F., Irmen, L., & Sczesny, S. (2007). Representation of the sexes in language. In K. Fiedler (Ed.), Social communication (pp. 163–187). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Stout, J. G., & Dasgupta, N. (2011). When he doesn’t mean you: Gender-exclusive language as ostracism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(6), 757–769. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211406434

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Switzer, J. Y. (1990). The impact of generic word choices: An empirical investigation of age- and sex-related differences. Sex Roles, 22, 69–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00288155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 371–394. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Tate, C. C., Ledbetter, J. N., & Youssef, C. P. (2013). A two-question method for assessing gender categories in the social and medical sciences. Journal of Sex Research, 50(8), 767–776. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2012.690110

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Tate, C. C., Youssef, C. P., & Bettergarcia, J. N. (2014). Integrating the study of transgender spectrum and cisgender experiences of self-categorization from a personality perspective. Review of General Psychology, 18(4), 302–312. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000019

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. The Associated Press. (2017). The associated press stylebook 2017. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Twenge, J. M., Campbell, W. K., & Gentile, B. (2012). Male and female pronoun use in US books reflects women’s status, 1900–2008. Sex Roles, 67, 488–493. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0194-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Van Anders, S. M. (2015). Beyond sexual orientation: Integrating gender/sex and diverse sexualities via sexual configurations theory. Archives Sex Behavior, 44, 1177–1213. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0490-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Vergoossen, H. P., Renström, E. A., Lindqvist, A., & Sendén, M. G. (2020). Four dimensions of criticism against gender-fair language. Sex Roles, 83(5), 328–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Webster, G. (2017). The Chicago manual of style (17th ed.). The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 748–762. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.748

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Casey McComb, Maevon Gumble, Celeste Tevis, Amanda Reichert, and Clare Mehta for their contributions to this project.

Funding

N/A.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Emily Keener.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

This research involved human participants and received Slippery Rock University IRB approval.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Participants were instructed to read the following job advertisement. Depending on which condition they were randomly assigned, they received one of three versions. The only difference between this and the other versions was the pronouns used. *The bolded pronouns were either non-binary (as shown here), binary (e.g., “he or she”), or masculine (“he/him/his”).

Our workforce is continually growing and thriving. As such, we are currently seeking part-time employees. On average, 71% of our workforce is comprised of part-time (compared to full-time) employees. Those who are hired are typically enthusiastic and bright; we usually know a good employee when we see *them. We are always striving to maintain a work environment that emphasizes a family culture, friendly faces, and an enjoyable atmosphere. We want each worker to feel as though they have the ability to communicate their ideas. When it comes to approaching a difficult task at work, we recognize the benefits of taking a more non-conventional approach.

Our staff is busy throughout the workday. What this means is that they need to be able to work in a fast-paced and energetic environment. However, we certainly do not want a part-time employee’s workload to interfere with their other responsibilities. As a part-time employee, each worker will devote 5–20 h of their weekly schedule to working. Also, each worker can work 7.5 h in 1 day, but they cannot work anymore than that. We accommodate each employee’s schedule around their other responsibilities.

We expect full employee support in fulfilling our goal to maintain a positive experience for the people who visit our facilities. Therefore, on a particularly busy day an employee may be asked to perform additional tasks during their shift to help fellow employees. Naturally, they will be highly praised for being able to adapt to the situation.

Finally, we expect and praise our employees who work as a team. We believe in praising employees who assume teamwork qualities. When we come across an outstanding worker, we believe rewarding them will boost the morale of the team. A worker is typically very pleased when their hard work is praised and the more they are praised, the harder they work! If this work environment sounds like a good fit for you, we encourage you to apply!

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Keener, E., Kotvas, K. Beyond He and She: Does the Singular Use of “They, Them, Their” Function Generically as Inclusive Pronouns for Cisgender Men and Women?. Gend. Issues 40, 23–43 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12147-022-09297-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12147-022-09297-8

Keywords

Navigation