Skip to main content
Log in

Optimism, pessimism, and false failure feedback: Effects on vigilance performance

  • Article
  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Following the logic of a prior experiment (Seligman et al., 1990) with varsity collegiate swimmers, sixty student volunteers performed a brief, but highly stressful vigilance task. Half were then given false feedback indicating poor performance; the others experienced a non-evaluative display at that point. All were then asked to repeat the vigil. Half the observers were assessed as high optimists and half as high pessimists. The pessimists showed a steeper vigilance decrement than the optimists, consistent with a model (Scheier and Carver, 1987) that proposes that pessimists are more emotion-focused under stress than optimists, and hence would be less attentive to the vigilance display, regardless of feedback condition. The false negative feedback actually turned out to be ineffective; hence, based on Seligman's account of the swimmer study, which requires effective negative feedback, there should have been no vigilance performance difference between optimists and pessimists, either pre- or post-feedback.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P., and Teasdale, J.D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87,49–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alluisi, E.A. (1969). Sustained performance. In E.A. Bilodeau and I.M. Bilodeau (Eds.), Principles of skill acquisition (pp. 59–102). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, A., Warm, J.S., and Dember, W.N. (1994). Specific and nonspecific transfer effects in training for vigilance. In M. Mouloua and R. Parasuraman (Eds.), Human performance in automated systems: Current research and trends (pp. 294–299). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berch, D.B. and Kanter, D.R. (1984). Individual differences. In J.S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 143–170). New York: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coren, S., Ward, L.M., and Enns, J.T. (1999). Sensation and perception (5th ed.), Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt-Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craig, A. (1984). Human engineering: The control of vigilance. In J.S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 247–292). New York: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, D.R. and Parasuraman, R. (1981). The psychology of vigilance. London, UK: Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Dember, W.N. and Brooks, J. (1989). A new instrument for measuring optimism and pessimism: Test-retest reliability and relations with happiness and religious commitment. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 365–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dember, W.N., Martin, S.H., Hummer, M.K., Howe, S.R., and Melton, R.S. (1989). The measurement of optimism and pessimism. Current Psychology: Research and Reviews, 8,102–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dember, W.N. and Warm, J.S. (1979) Psychology of perception (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dittmar, M.L., Warm, J.S., and Dember, W.N. (1985). Effects of knowledge of results on performance in successive and simultaneous vigilance tasks: A signal detection analysis. In R.E. Eberts and C.G. Eberts (Eds.), Trends in ergonomics/human factors II (pp. 195–202). New York, NY: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galinsky, T.L., Rosa, R.R., Warm, J.S., and Dember, W.N. (1993). Psychophysical determinants of stress in sustained attention. Human Factors, 35,603–614.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, P.A. and Warm, J.S. (1989). A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention. Human Factors, 31, 519–537.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, W.C. (1993). Engineering psychology in a changing world. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 231–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hiroto, D.S. (1974). Locus of control and learned helplessness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 187–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huey, B.M. and Wickens, C. (1993). Summary. In B.M. Huey and C. Wickens (Eds.) Workload transition: Implications for individual and team performance, (pp. 1–12). Washington D.C.: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummer, M.K., Dember, W.N., Melton, R.S., and Schefft, B.K. (1992). On the partial independence of optimism and pessimism. Current Psychology: Research and Review, 11, 37–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koelega, H.S. (1992). Extroversion and vigilance performance: 30 years of inconsistencies. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 239–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R.S. and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, L.M., Dember, W.N., Schefft, B.K., and Radenhausen, R.A. (1995). Can experimentally induced mood affect optimism and pessimism scores? Current Psychology: Research and Review, 14, 29–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackworth, J. (1970). Vigilance and attention. Baltimore, MD: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Joyner, L., Gilliland, K., Huggins, J., and Falconer, S. (1999). Validation of a comprehensive stress state questionnaire: Towards a state big three? In I. Merville, I.J. Deary, F. DeFruyt, and F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (vol. 7) pp. 335–350. Tilburg: Tilburg university Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, J.E. (Ed.). (1970). Social and psychological factors in stress. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., Pury, C.L., and Luten, A.G. (1995). Explanatory style in anxiety and depression. In G.M. Buchanan and M.E.P. Seligman (Eds.), Explanatory style, (pp. 135–158). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nachreiner, F. (1977). Experiments on the validity of vigilance experiments. In R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance and physiological correlates (pp. 665–678). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natali-Alemany, R. (1991). Moods, coping, and perception of daily life events: Are these factors related to optimism and pessimism? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cincinnati.

  • Parasuraman, R. (1986). Vigilance, monitoring, and search. In: K.R. Boff, L. Kaufman, and J.P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance, Vol II: Cognitive processes and performance (pp. 431–439). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C, Buchanan, G.M., and Seligman, M.E.P. (1995). Explanatory style: History and evolution of the field. In G.M. Buchanan and M.E.P. Seligman (Eds.), Explanatory style (pp. 1–20). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilley, S., Geers, A.L., Lindsay, D.L., Dember, W.N., and Schafer, J. (1998). Disentangling the importance of different optimism-pessimism instruments: Research issues and implications. Poster presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Society, Washington DC.

  • Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J.M. (1998). “Reality” is complicated. American Psychologist, 53, 673–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Satchel, P. (1993). Cockpit monitoring and alerting systems. Cambridge, UK: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scerbo, M.W. (1998). What's so boring about vigilance. In R.R. Hoffman, M.F. Sherrick and J.S. Warm (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 145–166). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M.F. and Carver, C.S. (1985) Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M.F. and Carver, C.S. (1987) Dispositional optimism and physical well-being: The influence of generalized outcome expectancies on health. Journal of Personality, 55,170–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P., Abramson, L., Semmel, A., and von Baeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 242–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P., and Maier, S.F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Thornton, N., and Thornton, K. (1990). Explanatory style as a mechanism of disappointing athletic performance. Psychological Science, 1,143–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P. and Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory style as a predictor of productivity and quitting among life insurance sales agents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 832–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, J.G., Warm, J.S., Dember, W.N., LaGrange, CM., and Matthews, G. (1996). Performance, workload, and stress in an abbreviated vigilance task. Poster presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Chicago, IL.

  • Temple, I.G., Dember, W.N., Warm, J.S., Jones, K.S., and LaGrange, C.M. (1997). The effects of caffeine on performance and stress in an abbreviated vigilance task. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 40,1293–1297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennen, H. and Eller, S.J. (1977). Attributional components of learned helplessness facilitation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 265–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warm, J.S. (Ed.) (1984). Sustained attention in human performance. Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warm, J.S. (1993). Vigilance and target detection. In B.M. Huey and C. Wickens (Eds.) Workload transition: Implications for individual and team performance (pp. 139–170). Washington D.C: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warm, J.S. and Dember, W.N. (1998). Tests of vigilance taxonomy. In R.R. Hoffman, M.F. Sherrick, and J.S. Warm (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 87–112). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Warm, J.S. and Jenson, H.J. (1984). The psychophysics of vigilance. In J.S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 15–59). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zullow, H.M. (1995). Pessimistic rumination in American politics and society. In G.M. Buchanan and M.E.P. Seligman (Eds.), Explanatory style (pp. 187–208). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Helton, W.S., Dember, W.N., Warm, J.S. et al. Optimism, pessimism, and false failure feedback: Effects on vigilance performance. Curr Psychol 18, 311–325 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-999-1006-2

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-999-1006-2

Keywords

Navigation