Advertisement

Current Psychology

, Volume 16, Issue 3–4, pp 181–224 | Cite as

Memory illusions and consciousness: Examining the phenomenology of true and false memories

  • James M. Lampinen
  • Jeffrey S. Neuschatz
  • David G. Payne
Article

Abstract

Several recent false memory studies have addressed the question of how false memories are experienced phenomenologically. In this article we argue that it is not only possible to address questions of this sort, but it is necessary for a full understanding of memory illusions. We define memory illusions as false beliefs about the past that are experienced as memories. We then review a corpus of evidence that addresses the phenomenological experience of false memories. None of this evidence is perfect, but taken in its totality it suggests that false memories are similar to true memories but are also subtly different. We conclude with several recommendations for how the phenomenology of false memories can be fruitfully explored.

Keywords

False Memory Current Psychology Source Monitoring Critical Lure Perceptual Illusion 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alba, J. W., & Hasher, L. (1983). Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulletin 93: 203–231.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, R. E. (1984). Did I do it or did I only imagine doing it? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113: 594–613.Google Scholar
  3. Anisfeld, M., & Knapp, M. (1968). Association, synonymity, and directionality in false recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology 77: 171–179.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Barclay, R. C, & DeCooke, P. A. (1988). Ordinary everyday memories: Some of the things which selves are made. In U. Neisser and E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering Reconsidered (pp. 91–125). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Barclay, R. C, & Wellman, H. M. (1986). Accuracies and inaccuracies in autobiographical memories. Journal of Memory and Language 25: 93–103.Google Scholar
  6. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Belli, R. F. (1989). Influences of postevent information: Misinformation interference and acceptance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 118: 72–85.Google Scholar
  8. Belli, R., & Loftus, E. F. (1994). Recovered memories of childhood abuse: A source monitoring perspective. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Dissociation: Clinical and Theoretical Perspectives (pp. 415–433). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  9. Bonto, M. B., & Payne, D. G. (1991). Role of environmental context in eyewitness memory. American Journal of Psychology 104: 117–134.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bouchard, E. (1932). Convicting the Innocent: Errors of Criminal Justice. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Bower, G.H., Black, J.B. & Turner, TJ. (1979). Scripts in memory for text. Cognitive Psychology 11: 177–220.Google Scholar
  12. Brainerd, C. J., & Mojardin. A. H. (1997).What is the nature of the verbatim traces? Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  13. Brainerd, C. J. & Reyna, V. F. (1990). Gist is the grist: Fuzzy-trace theory and the new intuitionism. Special Issue: Limited resource models of cognitive development. Developmental Review, 10, 3–47.Google Scholar
  14. Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1995). Autosuggestibility in memory development. Cognitive Psychology 28: 65–101.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Mojardin, A. H. (1996). Conjoint recognition. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  16. Bransford, J. D., Barclay, J. R., & Franks, J. J. (1972). Sentence memory: A constructive versus interpretive approach. Cognitive Psychology 3: 193–209.Google Scholar
  17. Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1971). The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Cognitive Psychology 2: 331–350.Google Scholar
  18. Brewer W. F. (1988). Memory for randomly sampled autobiographical events. In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering Reconsidered (pp. 21–90). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Brewer W. F. (1992). Phenomenal experience in laboratory and autobiographical memory tasks. In M. A. Conway, D. C. Rubin, H Spinnler and H. A. Wagenaar (Eds.), Theoretical Perspectives on Autobiographical Memory (pp. 31–52). Netherlands: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  20. Brewer, W. F., & Treyens, J. C. (1981). Role of schemata in memory for places. Cognitive Psychology 13: 207–230.Google Scholar
  21. Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5: 73–99.Google Scholar
  22. Bunuel, L. (1985). My Last Breath. London: Fontana.Google Scholar
  23. Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1993). Suggestibility of the child witness: A historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin 113:403–439.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Ceci, S. J., Crotteau-Huffman, M. L., & Smith E. (1994). Repeatedly thinking about a non-event: Source misattributions among preschoolers. Consciousness and Cognition 3: 388–407.Google Scholar
  25. Ceci, S. J., Ross, D. F., & Toglia, M. P. (1987). Suggestibility of children's memory: Psycholegal implications. Journal of Experimental Psychology General 116: 38–49.Google Scholar
  26. Ceci, S. J., Toglia, M. P., & Ross, D. F. (1990). Historical and cultural account of children's suggestibility. In R. Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.), Third Annual Emory Cognition Symposium: Remembering in Naturalistic Contexts (pp. 285–297). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Chambers, K. L., & Zaragoza, M. S. (1993). The effect of source credibility and delay on eyewitness suggestibility. Paper presented at the Thirty-Fourth Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Washington, D.C.Google Scholar
  28. Christiaansen, S. A. (1989). Flashbulb memories: Special, but not so special. Memory & Cognition 17: 435–443.Google Scholar
  29. Christiaansen, R. E.,& Ochalek, K. (1983). Editing misleading information from memory: Evidence for the coexistence of original and postevent information. Memory & Cognition 11:467–475.Google Scholar
  30. Conway, M. A. (1995). Flashbulb Memories. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  31. Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology 58: 17–22.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Dooling, D. J., & Christiaansen, R. E. (1977). Episodic and semantic aspects of memory for prose. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Learning and Memory 3:428–436.Google Scholar
  33. Dunning, D., & Stern, L. B. (1994). Distinguishing accurate from inaccurate eyewitness identifications via inquiries about decision processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67: 818–835.Google Scholar
  34. Egan, D. E. (1958). Recognition memory and the operating characteristic (Technical Note AFCRTN 58-51). Bloomington: Indiana University Hearing and Communication.Google Scholar
  35. Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1994). Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  36. Flanagan, O. (1992). Consciousness reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  37. Gallo, D. A., Roberts, M. J., & Seamon, J. G. (1997). Remembering words not presented in lists: Can we avoid creating false memories? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 4: 271–276.Google Scholar
  38. Gardiner, J. M.(1988). Functional aspects of recollective experience. Memory & Cognition 16: 309–313.Google Scholar
  39. Gardiner, J.M. & Java, R.I. (1990). Recollective experience in word and nonword recognition. Memory and Cognition 18: 23–30Google Scholar
  40. Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. I. (1993). Recognizing and remembering. In A. F. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway, & P. E. Morris, (Eds.), Theories of Memory (pp. 168–188). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  41. Goodman, G. S., & Reed, R. S. (1986). Age differences in eyewitness testimony. Law and Human Behavior 10: 317–332.Google Scholar
  42. Greene, E., Flynn, M. S., & Loftus, E. F. (1982). Inducing resistance to misleading information. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 21: 207–219.Google Scholar
  43. Hall J. W., & Kozloff, E. E. (1970). False recognition as a function of number of presentations. American Journal of Psychology 83: 272–279.Google Scholar
  44. Hasher, L., & Griffin, M. (1978). Reconstructive and reproductive processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Learning, and Memory 4: 318–330.Google Scholar
  45. Heimholte, H. (1971). The facts of perception. In R. Kahl (Ed.), Selected Writings of Herman von Helmholtz (p. 381). Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press.Google Scholar
  46. Hintzman, D. L. (1988). Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review 95: 528–551.Google Scholar
  47. Hirshman, E, & Master, S. (1997). Modeling the conscious correlates of recognition memory: Reflections on the remember-know paradigm. Memory & Cognition 25: 345–351.Google Scholar
  48. Huff, R., Rattner, A., & Sagarin, E. (1986). Guilty until proven innocent. Crime and Delinquency 32: 518–544.Google Scholar
  49. Hyman, I. E., Husband, T. H. & Billings, F. J. (1995). False memories of childhood experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology 9: 181–197.Google Scholar
  50. Hyman, I. E., & Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Language 35:101–117.Google Scholar
  51. Hyman, I. E., Wilkinson, C. L., & Thorn, C. L. (1997). Individual differences and false autobiographical memories. Paper presented at the Second Biennial Conference of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition (SARMAC), Toronto, Ontario.Google Scholar
  52. Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30: 513–541.Google Scholar
  53. Jacoby, L. L., Allan, L. G., Collins, J. C, & Larwill, L. K. (1988). Memory influences subjective experience: Noise judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 14: 240–247.Google Scholar
  54. Jacoby, L. L. & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 110: 306–340.Google Scholar
  55. Jacoby, L. L. Kelley, C. M., & Dywan, J. (1989). Memory attributions. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 391–421). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  56. Jacoby, L. L. & Whitehouse, K. (1989). An illusion of memory: False recognition influenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118: 126–135.Google Scholar
  57. Jacoby, L. L., Woloshyn, V., & Kelley, C. M. (1989). Becoming famous without being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory produced by dividing attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 118: 115–125.Google Scholar
  58. Johnson, M. K. (1985). The origin of memories. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances in Cognitive-behavioral Research and Therapy (pp. 1–27). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  59. Johnson, M. K. (1988). Discriminating the origin of information. In T. F. Oltmanns. &. B. A. Mahler (Eds.), Delusional Beliefs: Interdisciplinary Perspective (pp. 34–65). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  60. Johnson, M. K., Bransford, J. D., & Solomon, S. K. (1973). Memory for tacit implications of sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology 98: 203–205.Google Scholar
  61. Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G. & Raye, C. L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117: 371–376.Google Scholar
  62. Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin 114: 3–28.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological Review 88: 67–85.Google Scholar
  64. Johnson, M. K., Taylor, T. H. & Raye, C. L. (1977). Fact and fantasy: The effects of internally generated events on the apparent frequency of externally generated events. Memory & Cognition 5: 116–122.Google Scholar
  65. Kelley, C. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Adult egocentrism: Subjective experience versus analytic bases for judgment. Journal of Memory and Language 35: 157–175.Google Scholar
  66. Kendler, H. J. (1987). Historical Foundations of Modern Psychology. Pacific Grove, California: Brooks/ Cole Publishing.Google Scholar
  67. Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Memory metaphors and the real life/laboratory controversy: Correspondence versus storehouse conceptions of memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19:167–228.Google Scholar
  68. Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., Reiser, B. J. (1978). Visual images preserve metric spatial information: Evidence from studies of image scanning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 4: 47–60.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Lampinen, J. M. (1996). Recollections of things schematic: The influence of schemas on recollective experience. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois.Google Scholar
  70. Lampinen, J. M., & Smith, V. L. (1995). The incredible (and sometimes incredulous) child witness: Child eyewitnesses' sensitivity to source credibility cues. Journal of Applied Psychology 80: 621–627.Google Scholar
  71. Lampinen, J. M., Neuschatz, & J. S., Payne, D. G. (1997). Source attributions for false memories cannot be explained by demand characteristics. (Manuscript in preparation.)Google Scholar
  72. Lampinen, J. M. & Thapar, A. (1997). Just how real are qualities of the unreal? (Manuscript in preparation.)Google Scholar
  73. Lane, S. M., & Chambers, K. (1997). The effects of warnings on false memories. Paper presented at the Second Biennial Conference of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition (SARMAC), Toronto, Ontario.Google Scholar
  74. Lane, S. M., & Zaragoza, M. S. (1995). The recollective experience of crossmodality confusion errors. Memory & Cognition 23: 607–610.Google Scholar
  75. Lindsay, D. S. (1990). Misleading suggestions can impair eyewitnesses' ability to remember event details. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16: 1077–1083.Google Scholar
  76. Lindsay, D. S. (1994). Memory source monitoring and eyewitness testimony. In D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, and M. P. Toglia (Eds.), Adult Eyewitness Testimony (pp. 27–55). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  77. Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1989). The eyewitness suggestibility effect and memory for source. Memory & Cognition 17: 349–358.Google Scholar
  78. Lindsay, D. S., & Kelley, C. M. (1996). Creating illusions of familiarity in a cued recall remember/know paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language 35: 197–211.Google Scholar
  79. Lindsay, R. C. L., Wells, G. L., & Rumpel, C. (1981). Can people detect eyewitness identification accuracy within and across situations? Journal of Applied Psychology 66: 79–89.Google Scholar
  80. Linton, M. (1975). Memory for real-world events. In D. A. Norman & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Explorations in cognition (pp. 376–404). San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  81. Linton, M. (1982). Transformations of memory in everyday life. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Memory Observed: Remembering in Natural Contexts (pp. 77–91). San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
  82. Linton, M. (1986). Ways of Searching Contents of Memory. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 50–67). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  83. Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist 48: 518–537PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Loftus, E. F., & Davies, G.M. (1984). Distortions in the memory of children. Journal of Social Issues 40: 51–67.Google Scholar
  85. Loftus, E. F., Donders, K., Hoffman, H.G., & Schooler, J. W. (1989). Creating new memories that are quickly accessed and confidently held. Memory & Cognition 17: 607–616.Google Scholar
  86. Loftus, E.F., & Ketcham, K. (1994). The Myth of Repressed Memory: False Memories and Allegations of Sexual Abuse. New York: St. Martins.Google Scholar
  87. Loftus, E.F., Miller, D.G., & Burns, H.J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory: 4:19–31.Google Scholar
  88. Loftus, E.F., & Palmer, J.C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 13: 585–589.Google Scholar
  89. Loftus, E.F., & Pickrell, J.E. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals 25:270–275.Google Scholar
  90. Loftus, E.F., Schooler, J., & Wagenaar, W. (1985). The fate of memory: Comment on McCloskey and Zaragoza. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 114: 375–380.Google Scholar
  91. Luus, C. A. E., & Wells, G. L. (1994). The malleability of eyewitness confidence: Co-witnessed and perseverance effects. Journal of Applied Psychology 79: 714–721.Google Scholar
  92. Mandler, G. (1975). Consciousness: Respectable, useful, and probably necessary. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  93. Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological Review 67: 252–271.Google Scholar
  94. Mather, M., Henkel, L. A., & Johnson, M. K. (in press). Evaluating characteristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire compared. Memory & Cognition.Google Scholar
  95. Mitchell, T. R., & Richman, C. L. (1980). Confirmed reservations: Mental travel. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 6: 58–66.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. McCloskey, M., Wible, C. G. & Cohen, N. J. (1988). Is there a special flashbulb-memory mechanism? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117: 171–181.Google Scholar
  97. McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M.S. (1985a). Misleading postevent information and memory for events: Arguments and evidence against memory impairment hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 114: 1–16.Google Scholar
  98. McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, M.S. (1985b). Postevent information and memory: Reply to Loftus, Schooler, and Wagenaar. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 114: 381–387.Google Scholar
  99. McDermott, K. B. (1996).The persistence of false memories in list recall. Journal of Memory and Language 35: 212–230.Google Scholar
  100. Nakamura, G. V., Graesser, A. C, Zimmerman, J. A. & Riha, J.(1985). Script processing in a natural situation. Memory & Cognition 75:140–144.Google Scholar
  101. Neisser, U. (1981). John Dean's memory: A case study. Cognition 9: 1–22.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Neisser, U. (1982). Snapshots or benchmarks? In U. Neisser (Ed.), Memory Observed: Remembering in Natural Contexts. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
  103. Neisser, U. (1987). Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  104. Neisser, U., & Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flashbulbs: False recollections of hearing the news about Challenger. In E. Winograd & U. Neisser (Eds.), Accuracy and Affect in Recall: Studies of Flashbulb Memories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  105. Neuschatz, J. S., Payne, D. G., & Lampinen, J. M. (1997). Effects of warnings on the production of false memories. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  106. Norman, K. A., & Schacter, D. L. (in press) False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Memory & Cognition.Google Scholar
  107. Parkin, A. J., & Walter, B. (1992). Recollective experience, normal aging, and frontal dysfunction. Psychology and Aging 7: 290–298.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Parkin, A.J., & Russo, R.(1993). On the origin of functional differences in recollective experience. Memory 7:231–237.Google Scholar
  109. Payne, D. G. & Blackwell. J. M. (in press). Truth in memory from human memory: Caveat emptor. In S. J. Lynn & N. P. Spanos (Eds.), Truth in Memory. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  110. Payne, D. G., Elie C. J., Blackwell, J. M, & Neuschatz, J. S. (1996). Memory illusions: Recalling, Recollecting, and Recognizing events that never occurred. Journal of Memory and Language 35: 261–285.Google Scholar
  111. Payne, D. G., Lampinen, J. M., & Cordero, M. L. (1996). Remberances of things not passed: Further evidence concerning false memories. Paper presented at the Thirty-Seventh Annual Meeting of the Psychnomics Society, Chicago, Illinois.Google Scholar
  112. Payne, D. G., Neuschatz, J. S., Lampinen, J. M., & Lynn, S. J. (in press). Compelling memory illusions: The phenomenal qualities of false memories. Current Directions in Psychological Science.Google Scholar
  113. Pezdek, K., Whetstone, T., Reynolds, K., Askari, N. & Dougherty, T. (1989). Memory for real-world scenes: The role of consistency with schema expectation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 15: 587–595.Google Scholar
  114. Piaget, J. (1962). Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  115. Pigott, M. A., Brigham, J. C. & Bothwell, R. K. (1990). A field study on the relationship between quality of eyewitnesses' descriptions and identification accuracy. Journal of Police Science and Administration 17: 84–88.Google Scholar
  116. Rajaram, S. (1993). Remembering and knowing: Two means of access to the personal past. Memory & Cognition 21: 89–102.Google Scholar
  117. Rajaram, S. & Roediger, H. L. (1997). Remembering and knowing as states of consciousness during retrieval. In J. D. Cohen and J. W. Schooler (Eds.), Scientific Approaches to Consciousness (pp. 213–240). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  118. Raskin, D. C, & Esplin, P. W. (1991). Assessment of children's statements of sexual abuse. In J. Doris (Ed.), The Suggestibility of Children's Recollections (pp. 153–164). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  119. Read, J. D. (1996). From a passing thought to a false memory in 2 minutes: Confusing real and illusory events. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 3: 105–111.Google Scholar
  120. Reed, S. K., Hock, H., & Lockhead, G. R. (1983). Tacit knowledge and the effect of pattern configuration on mental scanning. Memory & Cognition 11: 137–143.Google Scholar
  121. Reisberg, D., (1996). Cognition: Exploring the Science of the Mind. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  122. Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1992). A fuzzy-trace theory of reasoning and remembering: Paradoxes, patterns, and parallelism. In A. F. Healy, S. A. Kosslyn, & R. M. Shiffrin (Eds.), From Learning Processes to Cognitive Processes: Essays in Honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 235–260). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  123. Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences 7: 1–75.Google Scholar
  124. Reyna, V. F., & Lloyd, F. (1997). Theories of false memories in children and adults. Unpublished manuscript, University of Arizona.Google Scholar
  125. Roediger, H. L. (1996). Memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language 35: 76–100.Google Scholar
  126. Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false Memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 803–814.Google Scholar
  127. Roediger, H. L., McDermott, K. B., & Goff, L. M. (1997). Recovery of true and false memories: Paradoxical effects of repeated testing. In M. A. Conway (Ed.), Recovered Memories and False Memories (pp. 118–149). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  128. Roediger, H. L., McDermott, K. B., & Robinson, K. J. (in press). The role of associative processes in creating false memories. In M. A. Conway, S. E. Gathercole, & C. Cornoldi (Eds.), Theories of Memory II. Hove, Sussex: Psychological Press.Google Scholar
  129. Schacter, D. L., Verfaellie, M., & Pradere, D. (1996). The neuropsychology of memory illusions: False recall and recognition in amnesic patients. Journal of Memory and Language 35: 319–334.Google Scholar
  130. Schooler, J. W., & Engstler-Schooler, T. Y. (1990). Verbal overshadowing and visual memories: Some things are left better unsaid. Cognitive Psychology 22: 36–71.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  131. Schooler, J. W., Gerhard, D. & Loftus, E. F. (1986). Qualities of the unreal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 12: 171–181.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Smith, M. E. (1993). Neurophysiological manifestations of recollective experience during recognition memory judgments. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5: 1–13.Google Scholar
  133. Smith, V. L., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1987). The social psychology of eyewitness accuracy: Misleading questions and communicator expertise. Journal of Applied Psychology 72: 294–300.Google Scholar
  134. Sporer, S. L., Penrod, S., Read, J. D., & Cutler, B. (1995). Choosing, confidence, and accuracy: A meta-analysis of the confidence-accuracy relation in eyewitness identification studies. Psychological Bulletin 118:315–327.Google Scholar
  135. Strack, F., & Forster, J. (1995). Reporting recollective experiences: Direct access to memory systems? Psychological Science 6: 352–358.Google Scholar
  136. Studebaker, C. A., Lampinen, J. M., Smith, V. L. (1993). Communicator expertise influences the encoding of misinformation. Paper presented at the Fifth Annual Convention of the American Psychological Society, Chicago, Illinois.Google Scholar
  137. Sulin, R. A., & Dooling, D. J. (1974). Intrusions of a thematic idea in retention of prose. Journal of Experimental Psychology 103: 255–262.Google Scholar
  138. Thompson, C. P., & Cowan, T. (1986). Flashbulb memories: A nicer interpretation of a Neisser recollection. Cognition 22: 199–200.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  139. Toglia, M.P., Ross, D.F., Ceci, S.J., & Hembrooke, H. (1992). Associates suggestibility of children's memory: A social-psychological and cognitive interpretation. In M. L. Howe, C. J. Brainerd, & V. F. Reyna (Eds.), Development of Long-term Retention (pp. 217–241). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  140. Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychologist 26: 1–12.Google Scholar
  141. Underwood, B. J. (1965). False recognition produced by implicit verbal responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology 70: 122–129.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  142. Wells, G. L. (1984). The psychology of lineup identifications. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 14: 89–103.Google Scholar
  143. Wells, G. L., Rydell, S. M., & Seelau, E. P. (1993). The selection of distracters for eyewitness lineups. Journal of Applied Psychology 78: 835–844.Google Scholar
  144. Wertheimer, M. (1912). Experimentelle stuidien uber das Sehen von Beuegung. Zeitschrift fuer Psychologie 61: (161–265).Google Scholar
  145. Wright, D. B. (1993). Recall of the Hillsboro disaster over time: Systematic biases of “flashbulb” memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology 7: 129–138.Google Scholar
  146. Zaragoza, M. S., & Koshmider, J. W. in (1989). Misled subjects may know more than their performance implies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15:246–255.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  147. Zaragoza, M. S., & Lane, S. M. (1994). Source misattributions and the suggestibility of eyewitness memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20: 934–945.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  148. Zaragoza, M. S. & Mitchell, K. J. (1996). Repeated exposure to suggestion and the creation of false memories. Psychological Science 7: 294–300.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • James M. Lampinen
    • 1
  • Jeffrey S. Neuschatz
    • 1
  • David G. Payne
    • 1
  1. 1.State University of New YorkBinghamton UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations