Advertisement

Guilt as a signal informing us of a threat to our morality

  • Yoshiya FurukawaEmail author
  • Ken’ichiro Nakashima
  • Ryota Tsukawaki
  • Yasuko Morinaga
Article
  • 68 Downloads

Abstract

Some studies have shown the possibility that people feel guilt not only due to interpersonal problems but also when experiencing threats to their own internal morality (e.g. Eskine et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(5), 947-950, 2013), whereas other studies have shown that guilt-induced behaviours can restore individuals’ sense of moral person (e.g. Gneezy et al. Management Science, 58(1), 179–187, 2012; Zhong et al. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(5), 859–862, 2010). These findings suggest that guilt can strongly reflect how much individuals deviate from what they perceive to be adequate moral person. Therefore, we proposed that guilt works as an alert system that signals people about threats to their morality. We used the Implicit Association Test (Greenwald et al. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464-1480, 1998) to investigate if an individual’s moral self decreased in the situation where that individual felt guilt. Results showed that implicit moral self in the guilt condition was lower than that in the control condition when controlling for individual variation in moral self. Our findings provide a new perspective on the function of guilt and generate new hypotheses about the relationship between guilt and behaviours.

Keywords

Guilt Implicit association test (IAT) Morality Moral self 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This paper was based partially on a doctoral dissertation submitted to the Hiroshima University by the first author and was supervised by the second author.

Funding

This work was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for JSPS Fellows (17 J05039) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science to the first author.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no other potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Supplementary material

12144_2019_144_MOESM1_ESM.docx (27 kb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 27 kb)
12144_2019_144_MOESM2_ESM.docx (29 kb)
ESM 2 (DOCX 28 kb)

References

  1. Aquino, K., & Reed, A., II. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1423–1440.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1423.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Azami, R. (2009). The relationship of the state humiliation, shame and guilt scale and trait shame and guilt scale. University of the Sacred Heart Papers, 31, 95–108.Google Scholar
  3. Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R., & Stewart, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe? Developing the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP) as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, 32(7), 169–177.Google Scholar
  4. Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 243–267.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1995). Personal narratives about guilt: Role in action control and interpersonal relationships. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17(1–2), 173–198.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.1995.9646138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brambilla, M., & Leach, C. W. (2014). On the importance of being moral: The distinctive role of morality in social judgment. Social Cognition, 32(4), 397–408.  https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2014.32.4.397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 253–264). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  8. Cougle, J. R., Goetz, A. R., Hawkins, K. A., & Fitch, K. E. (2012). Guilt and compulsive washing: Experimental tests of interrelationships. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(4), 358–366.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9359-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cryder, C. E., Springer, S., & Morewedge, C. K. (2012). Guilty feelings, targeted actions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(5), 607–618.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211435796.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A., & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 347–368.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014211.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Egloff, B., Schwerdtfeger, A., & Schmukle, S. C. (2005). Temporal stability of the implicit association test-anxiety. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84(1), 82–88.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8401_14.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Eskine, K. J., Novreske, A., & Richards, M. (2013). Moral contagion effects in everyday interpersonal encounters. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(5), 947–950.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.04.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Fujii, T., & Sawada, M. (2014). Relating self-esteem with schadenfreude using the implicit association test. Japanese Journal of Research on Emotions, 21(3), 114–123.  https://doi.org/10.4092/jsre.21.114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gino, F., Kouchaki, M., & Galinsky, A. D. (2015). The moral virtue of authenticity: How inauthenticity produces feelings of immorality and impurity. Psychological Science, 26(7), 983–996.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615575277.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Gneezy, A., Imas, A., Brown, A., Nelson, L. D., & Norton, M. I. (2012). Paying to be nice: Consistency and costly prosocial behavior. Management Science, 58(1), 179–187.  https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1110.1437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 17–41.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015575.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852–870). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Harashima, M., & Oguchi, T. (2007). Effects of explicit and implicit self-esteem on in-group favoritism. The Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(1), 69–77.  https://doi.org/10.2130/jjesp.47.69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hofmann, W., Wisneski, D. C., Brandt, M. J., & Skitka, L. J. (2014). Morality in everyday life. Science, 345(6202), 1340–1343.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1251560.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Gilovich, T., & Ariely, D. (2013). Moral masochism: On the connection between guilt and self-punishment. Emotion, 13(1), 14–18.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029749.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(1), 16–32.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.16.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Ketelaar, T., & Au, W. T. (2003). The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cognition and Emotion, 17(3), 429–453.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000662.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 633–644.  https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.45.6.633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Miller, D. T., & Effron, D. A. (2010). Psychological license: When it is needed and how it functions. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 115–155.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(10)43003-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Monin, B., & Jordan, A. H. (2009). The dynamic moral self: A social psychological perspective. In D. Narvaez & D. K. Lapsley (Eds.), Personality, identity, and character: Explorations in moral psychology (pp. 341–354). New York: Cambridge University Press.  https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511627125.016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 303–321.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014823.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Oshio, A., Nishino, T., & Hayamizu, T. (2009). Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancy and assumed competence. The Japanese Journal of Personality, 17(3), 250–260.  https://doi.org/10.2132/personality.17.250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., & Schroeder, D. A. (2005). Prosocial behavior: Multilevel perspectives. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 365–392.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070141.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Perugini, M., & Leone, L. (2009). Implicit self-concept and moral action. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(5), 747–754.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.03.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Riva, P., Brambilla, M., & Vaes, J. (2016). Bad guys suffer less (social pain): Moral status influences judgements of others’ social suffering. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55(1), 88–108.  https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12114.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Rothschild, Z. K., & Keefer, L. A. (2017). A cleansing fire: Moral outrage alleviates guilt and buffers threats to one’s moral identity. Motivation and Emotion, 41(2), 209–229.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9601-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Sakurai, S. (2000). Investigation of the Japanese version of Rosenberg’s self-esteem scale. Bulletin of Tsukuba Developmental and Clinical Psychology, 12, 65–71.Google Scholar
  36. Shimizu, H. (2016). An introduction to the statistical free software HAD: Suggestions to improve teaching, learning and practice data analysis. Journal of Media, Information and Communication, 1, 59–73.Google Scholar
  37. Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1359–1366.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417632.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Stewart, C., Rogers, F., Pilch, M., Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Westermann, S. (2017). The effect of social exclusion on state paranoia and explicit and implicit self-esteem in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 57, 62–69.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2017.04.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Strohminger, N., & Nichols, S. (2014). The essential moral self. Cognition, 131(1), 159–171.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345–372.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193–210.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Uebayashi, K., Tado’oka, Y., Ishii, K., & Murata, K. (2016). The effect of black or white clothing on self-perception of morality. The Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55(2), 130–138.  https://doi.org/10.2130/jjesp.si1-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Watanabe, E., & Ohtsubo, Y. (2012). Costly apology and self-punishment after an unintentional transgression. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3), 87–105.  https://doi.org/10.1556/JEP.10.2012.3.1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. West, C., & Zhong, C. B. (2015). Moral cleansing. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 221–225.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 748–762.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.748.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Xu, Z., & Ma, H. (2014). Moral identity in Chinese context: Construction and validation of an objective measure. Research on Humanities & Social Sciences, 4(2), 34–45.Google Scholar
  47. Xu, H., Bègue, L., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Too fatigued to care: Ego depletion, guilt, and prosocial behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(5), 1183–1186.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Xu, H., Bègue, L., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). Washing the guilt away: Effects of personal versus vicarious cleansing on guilty feelings and prosocial behavior. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(97).  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00097.
  49. Yamamoto, M., Matsui, Y., & Yamanari, Y. (1982). The structure of perceived aspects of self. The Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 30(1), 64–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2006). Feeling is for doing: A pragmatic approach to the study of emotions in economic behavior. In D. De Cremer, M. Zeelenberg, & J. K. Murnighan (Eds.), Social psychology and economics (pp. 117–137). New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  51. Zhang, H., Chen, S., Wang, R., Jiang, J., Xu, Y., & Zhao, H. (2017). How upward moral comparison influences prosocial behavioral intention: Examining the mediating role of guilt and the moderating role of moral identity. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(1554).  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01554.
  52. Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313(5792), 1451–1452.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1130726.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Zhong, C. B., Liljenquist, K., & Cain, D. M. (2009). Moral self-regulation: Licensing and compensation. In D. De Cremer (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on ethical behavior and decision making (pp. 75–89). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  54. Zhong, C. B., Strejcek, B., & Sivanathan, N. (2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(5), 859–862.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.04.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Yoshiya Furukawa
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Ken’ichiro Nakashima
    • 1
  • Ryota Tsukawaki
    • 3
  • Yasuko Morinaga
    • 1
  1. 1.The Graduate School of EducationHiroshima UniversityHigashi-HiroshimaJapan
  2. 2.Japan Society for the Promotion of ScienceTokyoJapan
  3. 3.Department of Social and Clinical PsychologyHijiyama UniversityHiroshimaJapan

Personalised recommendations