When saying that you are biased means that you are acurate? The moderating effect of cognitive structuring on relationship between metacognitive self and confirmation bias use

  • Yoram Bar-Tal
  • Hanna Brycz
  • Barbara Dolinska
  • Dariusz Dolinski
Article
  • 84 Downloads

Abstract

The aim of our study was to answer two questions: 1. How accurate are the reports of people who assert they are biased? 2. Why do people who know they are biased tend to engage in more inappropriate behavior? A total of 340 undergraduate students participated in the study. They followed a special procedure measuring cognitive structuring, efficacy to fulfill personal need to achieve cognitive structuring, metacognitive self (i.e. self-awareness of biases), and the level of performed confirmation bias. The procedure was created for investigating confirmation bias concerning the perception of self versus others. The first question may be answered by the assertion that the more metacognitive self-awareness of biases a person possesses, the more confirmation bias they exhibit. The pattern of results concerning the second issue demonstrates that only efficacy to fulfill personal need for structure (EFEN) moderates the relationship between metacognitive self and confirmation bias. Thus, only low-EFEN individuals perceived the extent of their use of biases more accurately. We explain the effect in terms of cognitive processing style: an individual inclination for piecemeal processes.

Keywords

Cognitive structuring Metacognition Confirmation bias Self versus other 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Ethical Approval

All procedures in studies involving human participants were performed in accordance with the ethical standards of institutional and national research committees, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individuals participating in the study.

Conflict of Interest

Yoram Bar-Tal declares that he has no conflict of interest. Hanna Brycz declares that she has no conflict of interest. Barbara Dolinska declares that she has no conflict of interest. Dariusz Dolinski declares that he has no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Argyle, M. (1992). The social psychology of everyday life. New York & London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  2. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy, toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bar-Tal, Y. (1994). The effect on mundane decision – making of need and ability to achieve cognitive structure. European Journal of Personality, 8, 45–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bar-Tal, Y. (2010). When the need for cognitive structure does not cause heuristic thinking: The moderating effect of the perceived ability to achieve cognitive structure. Psychology, 1, 96–105.  https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2010.12013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bar-Tal, Y., & Guinote, A. (2002). Who exhibits more stereotypical thinking? The Effect of need and ability to achieve cognitive structure on stereotyping. European Journal of Personality, 16, 313–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bar-Tal, Y., & Kossowska, M. (2010). Efficacy at fulfilling the need for closure: The construct and its measurment. In J. P. Villanueva (Ed.), Personality traits: Classifications, effects and changes (pp. 47–64). New York: Nova Science Publisher.Google Scholar
  7. Bar-Tal, Y., Kishon-Rabin, L., & Tabal, N. (1997). The effect of need and ability to achieve cognitive structuring on cognitive structuring. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1156–1178.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bar-Tal, Y., Shrira, A., & Keinan, G. (2013). The effect of stress on cognitive structuring: A cognitive motivational model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 87–99.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312461309.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Beer, N., & Moneta, G. B. (2010). Construct and concurrent validity of the Positive Metacognitions and Positive Meta – Emotions Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 977–982.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Blankenship, K. L., Nesbit, S. M., Murray, R. A., & Renee, A. (2013). Driving anger and metacognition: The role of thought confidence on anger and aggressive driving intentions. Aggressive Behavior, 39, 323–334.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21484.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. Srull & R. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 1–36). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Brycz, H. (2011). Perception accuracy of biases in self and in others. Psychology Research, 1, 203–215.Google Scholar
  13. Brycz, H., & Karasiewicz, K. (2011). Metacognition and self-regulation: the Metacognitive Self Scale. Acta Neuropsychologica, 9, 263–289.Google Scholar
  14. Brycz, H., Jurek, P., Pastwa-Wojciechowska, B., Peplińska, A., & Bidzan, M. (2014a). Self-attributions of meta-knowledge of the self in terms of Bernard Weiner’s theory. Przegląd Psychologiczny, 57, 347–367.Google Scholar
  15. Brycz, H., Wyszomirska-Góra, M., Bar-Tal, Y., & Wisniewski, P. (2014b). The effect of metacognitive self on confirmation bias revealed in relation to community and competence. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 45, 306–311.  https://doi.org/10.2478/ppb-2014-0037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Bunder, S. (1962). Intolerance of ambiguity as a personality variable. Journal of Personality, 30, 29–50.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1962.tb02303.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Chaiken, S., Libermen, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuation context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintendent Thoughts (pp. 212–252). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  18. Chaiken, S., Giner-Sorolla, R., & Chen, S. (1996). Beyond accuracy: Defense and impression motives in heuristic and systematic information processing. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 553–578). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  19. Deutsch, M. (1960). The pathetic fallacy. An observer error in social perception. Journal of Personality, 28, 317–332.Google Scholar
  20. Efklides, A. (2001). Metacognitive experiences in problem solving: Metacognition, motivation, and self- regulation. In A. Efklides, J. Kuhl, & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp. 297–323). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Efklides, A. (2008). Metacognition: Defining its facets and levels of functioning in relation to self-regulation and co-regulation. European Psychologist, 13, 277–287.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.13.4.277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fiske, S. (1993). Controlling other people. The impact of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 45, 621–628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fiske, S., & Linville, P. (1980). What does the schema concept buy us? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 543–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fiske, S., & Pavelchak, M. A. (1986). Category – based versus piecemeal- based affective responses. Developments in schemata-triggered affect. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 167–203). New York: Guliford Press.Google Scholar
  25. Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fletcher-Flinn, C., & Snelson, H. (1997). The relation between metalinguistic ability, social metacognition, and reading: A developmental study. New Zeland Journal of Psychology, 26, 20–28.Google Scholar
  27. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision making: A psychological analysis of conflict, choice, and commitment. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  29. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 273–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Konarski, R., & Brycz, H. (2017). Construct and concurrent validity of the positive metacognitions and the positive meta-emotions questionnaire in the Polish population. Sage Open doi.  https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244017705423.
  31. Kontos, S., & Nicholas, J. G. (1986). Independent problem solving in the development of metacognition. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 147, 481–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kossowska, M., & Bar-Tal, Y. (2013). Need for closer and heuristic information processing: The moderating role of the ability to achieve the need for closure. British Journal of Psychology, 104, 457–480.  https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12001.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Kruglanki, A., & Webster, D. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: Seizing and freezing. Psychological Review, 103, 263–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kruglanski, A. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational basis. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. McCormick, C. B. (2003). Metacognition and learning. In W. Reynolds, M. Weiner, & G. E. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (pp. 79–102). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
  36. Minor, K., & Lysaker, P. (2014). Necessary, but not sufficient: Links between neurocognition, social cognition, and metacognition in schizophrenia are moderated by disorganized symptoms. Schizophrenia Research, 159, 198–204.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.08.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Mitchell, J. P., Cloutier, J., Banaji, M. R., & Macrae, C. N. (2006). Medial prefrontal dissociations during processing of trait diagnostic and nondiagnostic person information. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 49–55.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Moore, B. S., Sherrod, D. R., Liu, T. J., & Underwood, B. (1979). The dispositional shift in attribution over time. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 553–569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Muluk, H., & Sumaktoyo, N. G. (2010). Intra- textual fundamentalism and the desire for simple cognitive structure: The moderating effect of the ability to achieve cognitive structure. Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 32, 1–22.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Neuberg, S., & Newsom, J. (1993). Personal need for structure: Individual differences in the desire for simple structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 113–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inferences – Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. New York: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  42. Otten, S., & Bar-Tal, Y. (2002). Self- anchoring in the minimal group paradigm: The impact of need and ability to achieve cognitive structuring. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 5, 265–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Parker, A., & Fischoff, B. (2008). Erratum: Can adolescents predict significant life events? Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 300–309.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.12.001.Google Scholar
  44. Swanson, K. M. (1990). Providing care in the NICU: Sometimes an act of love. Advances in Nursing Science, 1, 60–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Szczepanik, J., & Brycz, H. (2017). Metacognitive self and hedonic tone: Behavioral and neural correlates. Poster presented at Self and Identity EASP General Meeting. Grenada, Spain.Google Scholar
  46. Takana, Y., & Kusumi, T. (2007). The role of metacognition in critical thinking proces. Japanease Psychological Review, 50, 256–269.Google Scholar
  47. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective in mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Vohs, J. L., Lysaker, P. H., Francis, M. M., Hamm, J., Buck, K. D., Olesek, K., Outcalt, J., Dimaggio, G., Leonhardt, B., Liffick, E., Mehdiyoun, N., & Breier, A. (2014). Metacognition, social cognition, and symptoms in patients with first episode and prolonged psychoses. Schizophrenia Research, 153, 54–59.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.01.012.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Weiner, B. (2014). An anecdotical history of motivation. Review of Psychology, 57, 311–335.Google Scholar
  50. Weinstein, N. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806–820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Sackler Faculty of MedicineTel-Aviv UniversityTel AvivIsrael
  2. 2.Faculty of Social SciencesUniversity of GdanskGdanskPoland
  3. 3.Faculty of Social SciencesOpole UniversityOpolePoland
  4. 4.Faculty of Psychology in WroclawSWPS University of Social Sciences and HumanitiesWarszawaPoland

Personalised recommendations