Abstract
Compensatory Control Model suggests that, when deprived of personal control, people compensate this lack by bolstering their belief in a controlling God. However, this is not the only way of reacting to control deprivation as past literature also depicted that people sometimes become motivated to restore their personal control. We argued that people with highly independent self-construal would not be comfortable with enhancing external control in the absence of a personal one. We conducted an online experiment (N = 246) and the results indicated that control deprivation actually decreased, not increased, belief in a controlling God for people with independent self-construal. Such finding suggested that not everyone compensates for the lack of personal control by enhancing external control. Implications of the findings and potential limitations were discussed.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
Notes
As the current study as a part of a PhD thesis, there were also other dependent measures including FAD-Plus (Paulhus and Carey 2011), desirability of control (Burger and Cooper 1979), and harmony control (Morling and Fiske 1999) scales. But all of these measures came after belief in a controlling God scale in the procedure, thus it would be safe to argue that inclusion or exclusion of these variables in the experimental design would not alter the effect of control deprivation manipulation on belief in a controlling God.
When religiosity was not taken as a covariate, the interaction was still significant (F(1, 242) = 8.01, p = .005, ηp2 = .032).
An alternative way of investigating such interaction would be by conducting a moderated regression analysis. When independence and manipulation variables were centered and the interaction term was entered in the last step, the interaction was marginally significant (β = −.12, p = .057). Although the overall pattern was the same, 2 × 2 ANOVA was presented as the main analysis since it produced more clear-cut findings.
References
Ali, A. J., Camp, R. C., & Gibbs, M. (2015). The concept of “free agency” in monotheistic religions: Implications for global businesses. Journal of Business Ethics, 60, 103–112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-005-5749-x.
Avtgis, T. Ä., & Rancer, A. S. (1997). Argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness as a function of locus of control. Communication Research Reports, 14(4), 441–450. https://doi.org/10.1080/08824099709388687.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic.
Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological reactance: A theory of freedom and control. New York: Academic.
Burger, J. M., & Cooper, H. M. (1979). The desirability of control. Motivation and Emotion, 3, 381–393. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00994052.
Hofstede, G. H. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
Kay, A. C., & Eibach, R. P. (2013). Compensatory control and its implications for ideological extremism. Journal of Social Issues, 69(3), 564–585. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12029.
Kay, A. C., & Friesen, J. (2011). On social stability and social change: Understanding when system justification does and does not occur. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(6), 360–364. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411422059.
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callan, M. J., & Laurin, K. (2008). God and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 18–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.18.
Kay, A. C., Whitson, J. A., Gaucher, D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Compensatory control: Achieving order through the mind, our institutions and the heavens. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 264–268. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01649.x.
Kay, A. C., Shepherd, S., Blatz, C. W., Chua, S. N., & Galinsky, A. D. (2010a). For god (or) country: The hydraulic relation between government instability and belief in religious sources of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 725–739. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021140.
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., McGregor, I., & Nash, K. (2010b). Religious belief as compensatory control. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1), 37–48. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309353750.
Kay, A. C., Moscovitch, D. A., & Laurin, K. (2010c). Randomness, attributions of arousal, and belief in god. Psychological Science, 21(2), 216–218. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609357750.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Models of agency: Sociocultural diversity in the construction of action. In V. M. Berman & J. J. Berman (Eds.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on the self (Vol. 49, pp. 1–57). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Morling, B., & Fiske, S. T. (1999). Defining and measuring harmony control. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 379–414. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1999.2254.
Paulhus, D. L., & Carey, J. M. (2011). The FAD-Plus: measuring lay beliefs regarding free will and related constructs. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 96–104. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2010.528483.
Pittman, T. S., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1989). Motivation and cognition: Control deprivation and the nature of subsequent information processing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25(6), 465–480. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(89)90001-2.
Pittman, T. S., & Heller, J. F. (1987). Social motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 461–489.
Rutjens, B. T., & Loseman, A. (2010). The society-supporting self: System justification and cultural worldview defense as different forms of self-regulation. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(2), 241–250. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430209351703.
Shepherd, S., Kay, A. C., Landau, M. J., & Keefer, L. A. (2011). Evidence for the specificity of control motivations in worldview defense: Distinguishing compensatory control from uncertainty management and terror management processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 949–958. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.026.
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5), 580–591. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205014.
Warburton, W. A., Williams, K. D., & Cairns, D. R. (2006). When ostracism leads to aggression: The moderating effects of control deprivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2), 213–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.005.
Wasti, A., & Erdil, S. E. (2007). Bireycilik ve toplulukçuluk değerlerinin ölçülmesi: Benlik kurgusu (Self-Construal Scale; SCS) ve INDCOL ölçeklerinin Türkçe geçerlemesi [Measurement of individualism and collectivism: Validation of Self Construal Scale and INDCOL in Turkish]. Yönetim Araştırmaları Dergisi, 7, 39–66.
Weber, M. (2002). The Protestant ethic and the" spirit" of capitalism and other writings. London: Penguin.
Zhou, X., He, L., Yang, Q., Lao, J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Control deprivation and styles of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 460–478. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026316.
Acknowledgements
This study was a part of the PhD thesis of Sinan Alper and it was supported by The Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey (TÜBİTAK) as a part of graduate scholarship programme (2211-A).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Conflict of Interest
On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
Informed Consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Alper, S., Sümer, N. Control Deprivation Decreases, Not Increases, Belief in a Controlling God for People with Independent Self-Construal. Curr Psychol 38, 1490–1494 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9710-9
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9710-9
Keywords
- Control deprivation
- Compensatory control
- Belief in god
- Independence